|
In Defence of Rigorous Subjectivityby Matthijs Cornelissen IntroductionSince the European
Enlightenment, science has laid a heavy stress on objectivity, and
for much of that time subjectivity has been suspect, particularly in
psychology. Though positivism is not a philosophical system that has
many supporters anymore, most hard science research is still
conducted as if there was a simple, stable world “out there”,
independent of the observer, waiting to be studied objectively. In
the hard sciences, objective, empirical research has produced such an
enormous, detailed and sophisticated body of knowledge and such an
astounding flood of eminently practical devices and applications,
that philosophical doubts and concerns about the theoretical
possibility of truly objective knowledge tend to be swept aside by
the pragmatic argument that what works so well must be true. As a
result, objectivity is valued highly and as long as research is
“evidence-based”, its scientific credentials can hardly be
contested. In
the social sciences, and especially psychology, this stress on
objectivity has come, however, at a high cost. In this more subtle
and complicated terrain, objective studies tend to produce results
that are, as one elderly British psychologist once remarked, either
trivial or dubious1.
In psychology especially, the present stress on objectivity seems to
have led to a trade-off between objectivity and reliability on the
one hand and validity, relevance, subtlety, and depth on the other.
Social constructionism, as perhaps the most prominent challenger of
the possibility of objective research, at least in the social
sciences, has helped to make researchers more aware of their social,
cultural and economic biases, but it has not been able to make much
of a positive contribution of its own. When applied to itself, it can
be charged with shooting itself in the foot and its adherents run the
danger of getting lost in narratives about narratives that float
freely in the emptiness of their own brilliance. If Feyerabend is
right that “anything goes” (Feyerabend,
1975, quoted in Skinner 1985),
then practical men shrug their shoulders and get back to work. But
for those who are interested in the more subtle aspects of the human
mind, that work can be frustrating. While excellent research is done
on the peripheries of psychology, especially in neurophysiology and
brain chemistry, the subtle inner workings of our own nature, our
consciousness, all that makes us uniquely human and that could make a
well-justified claim on being the core territory of psychology seems
to escape serious research. If we try to be academically solid, the
subtlety and depth tend to get lost, and if we insist on staying with
the subtleties, we end up with qualitative research that tends to
lack the required level of certainty and precision. This
is not surprising, because the subject matter of psychology is not
visible “out there” but hidden “inside”, and so, as long as
we conduct psychology as an objective, third person science, we are
dependent on lay-subjects for collecting the data. As I argued
elsewhere (2001) such attempts are like trying to develop astronomy
on the basis of what members of the general public report during
their evening stroll. The main argument for using what lay-people
report in questionnaires and interviews is that people are supposed
to have privileged access to their own mental processes, and so if we
want to study others, we can hardly avoid consulting them on what
happens in their minds. As such this is the right method for
population surveys that have no other objective than to map what
different people report about themselves, but it is not a very good
way to find out how the human mind actually works. As many 19th century psychologists already knew, and the Freudians and cognitive
behaviourists rediscovered in their own different ways, most of what
happens in our minds remains below the threshold of awareness. The
ordinary waking consciousness has access to only a tiny fraction of
all that goes on in our minds. Research that is dependent on lay
self-reports is thus intrinsically limited to surface mental
phenomena, and due to all kinds of social pressures and conflicts of
interest, it is not very good at even that. The vast majority of
qualitative methods and post-modern tools for the analysis of
narratives don't help much in this respect, because though they do
add detail and a certain refinement, they are still limited to the
surface. The harsh reality is that “privileged” access does not
necessarily imply “accurate” or “reliable” access, and there
are a number of studies pointing to people's poor record at giving
accurate accounts about themselves. In the end, no amount of
sophistication at the level of analysis and interpretation can
compensate for poor quality of the original data, and unless its data
gathering itself becomes more sophisticated, psychology is unlikely
to make any substantial progress. It
is at this point that the Indian tradition can make one of its most
valuable contributions. It has tackled the difficulties of subjective
investigation in a manner that is totally different from the Western
approach, but that is logically coherent and extremely effective in
terms of the insight, mastery (and happiness) that it produces in the
practitioner. Given
how much psychology could gain from the type of unbiased, reliable,
penetrating, detailed insights into human consciousness that the
Indian tradition has developed during the last several millennia, one
might wonder why its basic methodology has not been accepted as part
of the scientific canon earlier, for there is access to good English
texts on the Indian tradition since more than a century, and many
Western scientists have used Indian practices like yoga for their own
growth and well-being. Part of the resistance to taking up these
methods might well be due to remnants of the amazing Western sense of
civilisational superiority, or perhaps even to simple parochialism.
Parochialism is tempting in science because it is so much easier to
study a foreign culture from the outside than from the inside. So it
is not really surprising that this is what the vast majority of
Western Sanskritists and Orientalists seem to have done in their
study of the various strands of Indian thought and practice. These
subjects have typically been studied to find out how “others”
behave and what these “others” think about reality. The outcome
of such studies is that one widens one's view of humanity's
possibilities, and while this feels good, it comes at very little
cost in terms of one's own certainties. To enjoy Indian music,
dance, and literature, to study Indian philosophy and even to engage
in hathayoga and mindfulness exercises (or for the therapists, to
prescribe these to their clients) is good for the individual and does
not fundamentally endanger our collective way of looking at ourselves
and the world. One can find this same outsider's view in almost all
meditation research, which till date has focused on psychological and
physiological (side-)effects of meditation that are easy to study
from within the existing scientific paradigm, but that are hardly
related to the reasons people meditate in the culture of origin
(Murphy & Donovan, 1997). Similarly, where non-Western
spirituality has entered medical training and practice it has almost
always been introduced on the clients' side, but only rarely on the
side of the medical practitioners. Entering
within a foreign culture and assessing human nature and one's own
psychological and cultural reality with methods that are deeply
embedded in a totally different worldview is a far more difficult
proposal. A serious, even-handed cross-cultural exchange between the
Indian and the Euro-American tradition about psychology would put
into question each side's most deeply held assumptions about the
nature of reality, the aim of life, the role of the individual and
even the divine. It appears that in spite of these difficulties quite
a large number of Western therapists have experienced and accepted
the value of Indian perspectives and practices in their work and
private lives, but as a collective enterprise, science has shown very
little inclination to adopt the methodologies that the Indian
tradition has developed to arrive at psychological knowledge. It is
interesting to speculate on the reasons for this dichotomy. One
reason could be that science is a highly privileged social activity
and by far the most widely trusted knowledge system for the conduct
of our collective life: It has a virtual monopoly as advisor for
government and it is the sole provider of content and method for
education in a very large part of the world. As a consequence the
scientific establishment is understandably hesitant to allow
tinkering with the methods and ideas that till now have functioned as
guarantors to this privileged and trusted position. In response I'd
argue that the sense of responsibility cuts two ways. If we agree
that a better understanding of our own subjective being is crucial
not only for the individual but also for the safety and progress of
society as a whole, then one might also wonder whether it is not
irresponsible to limit ourselves to objective research in the
subjective domain, as doing so has all the trappings of Nasruddin's
error.2 There
is another hurdle in the way of adoption of Indian methods of inquiry
which might be more difficult to overcome. The Indian methods of
looking within tend to produce, not in everybody but still rather
frequently, what one could call “mystical” experiences,
experiences that at least appear to deal with non-physical realities,
or that evoke in the observer the sense of being in contact with
something sublime, absolute, or even divine. Within the Indian
tradition this is no problem, as it has always looked at the world as
a complex integrated whole in which spirit is the essence of matter,
and matter one of the many manifestations of spirit. But within the
Euro-American tradition this is a problematic and often emotionally
loaded issue: Religion and science have widely been considered
separate domains, and spiritual experiences are commonly held to be
part of religion. The conflation of spirituality and religion on the
one hand, and the stark separation of science and religion on the
other has deep roots in the history of Europe but it is not a
universal given. There are many strands of the Indian yoga tradition
that rely primarily on experience and the rigorous application of
sophisticated methods, and that have even in their decentralised
social structure, more in common with modern science than with the
Abrahamic religions with their dependence on the authority of a
single, historical revelation. All
this being as it may, it would be a gross error to underestimate the
difficulties involved in subjective science, and the hesitancy to
leave the safe domain of objectivity that has produced so many clear
benefits to humanity is understandable, but, as I'll try to argue in
this article, I do not think that it is either necessary, or
excusable to leave the subjective domain outside science. It is not
necessary because humanity actually has developed rigorous methods to
arrive at valid and reliable subjective knowledge. It is also not
excusable because the subjective domain contains virtually everything
that really matters to people: love, joy, experience, beauty, will,
values, meaning, and even knowledge, which in last reckoning depends
on someone's subjective consciousness to be initiated, understood and
enjoyed. The
Indian tradition has perhaps more than any other put effort into the
development of rigorous subjective enquiry. The very first section of
one of its oldest and most highly respected texts, the Rig Veda deals
with it,3 and so do the vast majority of the major Upanishads. In fact, one
could well look at the entire school of jnanayoga as a method to arrive at valid knowledge. It is clearly beyond the
scope of this article to do justice to this vast literature, and all
I'll try to do here is to indicate the broad principles involved in
two comparatively straightforward methods of Vedantic psychological
inquiry, after which I'll make a few observations about the nature
of reality that seem to follow from the application of these methods.
I'll start with the methods, as the different view of reality
becomes visible when these methods are followed, though I am aware
that there is a chicken and egg problem here, and one suspects that
most people would need to feel at least a basic intuitive sympathy
for the worldview before they would seriously follow the methods. The
central difference between the broad thrust of traditional Western
and Eastern approaches to inquiry I have already alluded to: instead
of resorting to objective studies of others, the Indian tradition has
put its effort into perfecting the subjective study of oneself. For
the collection of its basic data it relies on a combination of two
interrelated processes, which we can describe as 1) a shifting the
borders of the observing self inwards, and 2) a cleaning up of the
instrumental nature. It may be noted in this context that in the
ordinary waking consciousness the mind is used to construct knowledge
on the basis of what comes in through the senses, aided on the one
hand by all kind of technical devices, and on the other by the
intellect. This is the part of the story that has been developed so
successfully by modern science. What is suggested here is that to
make a similar progress in psychology, we need to develop a pure
witness consciousness that can gradually gain direct access to the
deepest layers of one's consciousness.4 The instrumental nature is then used subsequently to express with a
gradually increasing perfection the knowledge arrived at by means of
direct inner seeing and hearing, drsti and shruti.
As the image goes, a wild stream cannot reflect the moon; an
absolutely still lake can.
Shifting the borders of the observing “I” inwardsOne
of the lines which the Indian tradition has followed to tackle the
notorious unreliability of first person perceptions is the idea that
the more detached we are, the more likely it is that we will see
things as they really are. According to this line of thought, the
core of the human predicament is that our consciousness identifies
itself with a little chunk of thinking, living matter, which it
erroneously thinks of as itself. In both Hindu and Buddhist
traditions this identification with an intrinsically small,
vulnerable and ultimately mortal creature is thus held up as the main
reason that the human being suffers and cannot see reality as it is.
One might think that this involvement and this identification, which
seems to have arisen from our evolutionary past, is actually useful
and even essential to our survival, but what the Indian tradition has
found is that at least for us humans this appears not to be the case
anymore. To get rid of the various identifications is obviously not
easy because in our ordinary waking state our consciousness is
completely entangled with our thoughts, feelings, intentions and
actions, but if one manages, the resulting detachment does not seem
to impair one's social functioning. In fact it seems rather to
enhance it: The Buddha, as an extreme example of one who realised and
preached one of the most stringent forms of detachment and championed
a technique that is aimed at the realisation that there actually is not even any self that can get attached to anything, may well have
been one of the most influential individuals in the history of
humanity. In a similar dialectic, if one frees one's consciousness
both from external content and ego-sense, it does not become small,
dull, and ultimately unconscious (as Jung thought5)
but it actually widens and becomes more intense, luminous and joyous
to a degree entirely unimaginable in our ordinary state. It appears
then that to the extent that one frees one's consciousness from the
small individual ego activities, it can become one with a far wider,
purer, more powerful form of consciousness that is experienced as
upholding, inhabiting and transcending everything in the universe,
and the resulting inner freedom and equanimity help to align one's
life with the larger movements of the cosmos in a manner that allows
a degree of effective and wholesome “right action” not available
to the ordinary ego-bound awareness. While
in modern times yoga is often regarded only from its soteriological
aspect, within the Indian tradition yoga is also, and perhaps even
primarily, seen as a way to arrive at reliable knowledge. In fact, in
the Indian tradition the two are insolubly linked: ignorance is seen
as the main cause of suffering, and it is held that the wiser one
becomes, the closer one comes to a lived experience of a subtle, but
intense and situation-independent Joy, Ananda, that is then experienced as an essential element of reality as it is
in itself. What stands in the way of both the unbiased seeing and the
unhampered enjoyment of reality, is one's involvement with a small,
separated and seemingly independent “ego” and its struggles for
survival, possession, etc. Yoga could, in this context, be described
as a double method to effect first the disentanglement of our
consciousness from its identification with the little, constructed
ego, and then its re-alignment, or even merger, with a much larger
consciousness that seems to uphold, inhabit and transcend the
Universe. Whether such a vast cosmic consciousness actually exists may be beyond the scope of psychology at present, but the manner by which attachment distorts perception is fairly straightforward. A few typical examples will make clear how different types of attachment can lead to different types of distortion.
In short, our
observations are coloured by our fears and desires, our physical
limitations and our mental preferences. Collectively we have put a
colossal and extremely successful effort into overcoming these
limitations in the physical domain: right from primary school,
children get a basic training in the foundations of mathematics and
science, and the most gifted are encouraged to pursue these lines.
But strangely enough we have not yet made a comparable effort in the
subjective domain. Every child learns to master complicated external
tools like cars and cell-phones, but there is hardly anyone who knows
how to silence his own mind, or who can consciously choose his own
emotions. The
Indian tradition has addressed this inner half of the equation and
asserts that it is actually possible to separate one's
consciousness from all distorting
factors.6 One of the techniques the Indian tradition uses to free itself from
the distorting ballast is to shift the boundary of the observing “I”
inwards. That this border can be shifted is nothing new.
Unconsciously we shift the borders of our “I” all the time: when
I watch a cricket match I may identify with city or country; at other
times I may represent my office or family; when I think I identify
with my mind; when I feel love I identify with my heart; when I feel
tired, I identify with my body; and I can also disidentify from all
of these and watch them semi-objectively, as if from the outside. For
intellectuals, the most difficult, and interesting assignment tends
to be the separation from their thoughts. Intellectuals typically
identify with them and Descartes may well have spoken for the entire
academic community when he made his very existence dependent on his
ability to think. It
is not easy to break the connection with one's thinking and as long
as we look at ourselves, semi-objectively through ordinary
introspection, we haven't shifted the border inwards far enough: In
ordinary introspection we think with one bit of our mind about some
other part of the mind, and we produce a running commentary in the
process. As long as we identify with this commentator we still
identify with one of the many processes that happen in the mind. What
is required is to go further inside and watch in
absolute silence this
commentary as well as all other things that happen to be going on in
one's consciousness without the slightest response or reaction. It
is, in a way, quite amazing that this has been found to be possible,
but it is: though it is not easy, it is doable to evolve a pure,
silent witness consciousness, sakshi.
From a developmental viewpoint it is interesting to note that there
is a gradient from full involvement in the activities of one's mind
to complete inner freedom. Initially one may still identify with the
sensations, feelings, and thoughts that are taking place in the mind,
while in the background there may be a growing awareness of a peace,
a stillness, a luminous space in which these activities are
happening. In the next phase once begins increasingly to identity
with that peace, that inner emptiness, while one is still also
somewhat sideways involved in the happenings in front. In itself this
is already a considerable progress towards unbiased observation, but
it is not far enough: the peace is still held with effort, a trace of
artificiality, and it can be disturbed at any moment so that one gets
entangled again in some egoic movement. There is even the possibility
of a kind of dull silence to intervene in between. But if one
persists, refusing any degree of identification or smallness, there
comes a definite turning point, a sudden moment at which there is
absolutely no involvement any more. Interestingly, a single event of
this type tends to leave a permanent change in one's basic “feel”
about life, reality and one's own role in it. After this “reversal
of consciousness”, one is in principle a free intelligence; one
has, so to say, no axe to grind anymore. In practice, however, life
is not that simple. Human nature is exceedingly complex, and the
“liberation” may involve more or less of different parts of one's
nature. As a result, even while there is a freedom at the core of
one's being, traces of egoism will still be there in other parts
and they may actually dominate one's behaviour. As a consequence,
liberation, or mukti,
is not enough to turn one's nature into a reliable instrument for
psychological observation. Sources of bias have to be removed even
from the rest of one's nature, and this is a long and laborious
process.
Cleaning up the “inner instrument” or antahkarana If
one wants to use the witness consciousness for the development of
psychology (or for any other practical purpose for that matter)
detachment of one's centre of consciousness from one's outer
nature is an essential, but not sufficient condition. One also needs
a pure “inner instrument”, antahkarana:
first to find out and then to express the more subtle events that are
going on inside one's consciousness. It is possible to distinguish
three distinct stages in the progressive process of perfecting human
nature into a good instrument of knowledge: the purification as
preparation for realisation; the
adjustment of the inner and outer nature as an immediate and
automatic consequence of the central realisation; and
finally the complete transformation of the nature that, if one puts
sufficient effort into it, can follow on the inner realisation. There are three major reasons why almost all yogic disciplines insist on an extensive outer discipline as preparation for the yogic practices proper. The first reason is simply that the inner freedom can be reached most easily when the outer nature is in a relatively harmonious, sattvic, state. The second reason is that if realisation does happen while some parts of the nature are not yet in harmony with the new inner status, the fundamental change in how one experiences one's essential Self can lead to a serious psychological imbalance. The third and final reason is that the powers, which the realisation and the path towards it inevitably bring about, can be dangerous if there are unregenerate impulses in the rest of the nature that misuse them. The painful frequency in the US of “spiritual emergencies” seems to be at least partially due to the use of decontextualised and poorly guided spiritual practices. Within the Indian tradition, powerful “techniques” for mediation are rarely stressed, and tend to be given only after many years of subtle psychological preparation. If you want to drive fast, you better know how to drive well. Interestingly, this purification of the outer nature is not absolutely indispensable: It does happen that a full inner liberation is achieved, while the outer nature is still in some sort of a mess, and it is not unusual that the inner realisation increases whatever remaining disharmonies there are in the outer nature: there is then a blissful inner freedom in the midst of an abysmal chaos outside. More commonly however, the inner realisation brings about a positive change at least in the more subtle inner parts of the nature: a greater gentleness, patience, light, love, and an imperturbable will for the welfare and growth of other people dominates the character. Occasionally both results coincide and the inner realisation leads to a beautiful inner gentleness combined with a rough and chaotic outer behaviour. For work in the world, and definitely for a fruitful study of psychological processes this obviously will not do. If we want that, then it is needed to purify and harmonise the outer nature as well as the inner essence.
Beyond the flatlanders' slit As
one carries the detachment further and learns to watch one's inner
nature and outer life with an increasingly pure consciousness, one is
likely to discover many aspects of the fabulous complexity of reality
that are hidden from the ordinary waking consciousness. Though the
various Indian schools of thought have worked out in quite different
ways how exactly the world-as-we-see-it arises from the interaction
between the Self and Nature, there is a fairly well-defined common
background understanding of reality which explains both the
communality and the differences between the various ways people see
and live “their” particular reality. One of the important
elements of this common understanding is the idea that there are
several different types of conscious existence that though ultimately
one in their essence, are entirely different in their manifestation.
Contemporary science has paid very little attention to these
different modes of being conscious, and in consciousness research the
subject is often presented as if our ordinary waking consciousness
were the only type of consciousness available. In the Indian systems
that are based on the methodology of self-observation discussed
above, the ordinary waking state is seen as not more than one type of
consciousness near the middle of a long scale of different types of
consciousness, somewhat like our visual range covers just some tiny
part of the enormous range of different types of light that exist in
nature. Material objects, feelings, volitions and ideas are all seen
as manifestations of conscious existence, and there are supposed to
exist all kinds of subtle phenomena besides, that our fixation on the
material world prevents us from perceiving. The
English language contains hints that in folk-psychology there is a
basic awareness of these different modes of being conscious within
the ordinary human range: We speak of “using our head” when we
want to think more clearly; of “opening our heart” when we need
more compassion; and of “trusting our gut-feelings” when
immediate survival is at stake. These are very crude distinctions,
however, compared to what is available on these issues within the
Indian tradition, and to what is available to direct inner sight once
we silence the mind and free our consciousness from its slavish
involvement in the incessant hum of our senses, desires, intentions
and thoughts. Charles C. Tart is one of those who have tried to
introduce some of this wider range into academia with his proposal
for “state specific sciences” (1972). However, his decision to
keep his theory largely separate from the enormous amount of work
that has already been done in this area by the various spiritual
traditions, and perhaps also the fact that much of the insights he
cites are derived from the chaotic world of drug-induced alternative
states, have arguably deprived his theory of sufficient concreteness,
internal harmony and complexity. It does not seem to have been taken
up and developed to any great extent in the 40 years since it was
first published. Perhaps it came simply too early. If we can use once more the development of astronomy as a metaphor for the development of psychology, then our mainstream academic psychology seems to compare to the Indian tradition somewhat like pre-Copernican to Einsteinian astronomy. Just as Ptolemy took the Earth as the centre of the physical universe, mainstream psychology takes the ordinary waking state (OWS) as if it were the one and only standard of all things psychological, everything else in this wondrous universe is judged exclusively from that, very limited, OWS-centred perspective.
Limiting itself to how the world appears to humans in their Ordinary Waking State, 19th century physics and 20th century psychology took it for granted that the world consists primarily, if not exclusively, of a space-time continuum in which material objects move and interact. Theoretical physicists have long ago begun to doubt that this is a very good model of reality, but it is still implicit in much of social science, and even in consciousness studies where it stands squarely in the way of serious progress. There is nothing like a real consensus in the field of consciousness studies, but the idea that consciousness “emerges” at a certain level of neurological complexity is presently so widely accepted that even those who differ from the mainstream tend to use it as the baseline from which they differentiate their own theory. What seems to escape attention is that “emergence” is not at all a valid explanatory category; it is in fact no more than an admission of ignorance. To stay within our metaphor, astronomy would have reached nowhere if it had simply accepted that the sun “emerges” every morning out of the Eastern horizon. In fact, “emergence” typically reminds one of what happens to Flatlanders when a 3-dimensional object passes through their two-dimensional space, and it is tempting to think that what is missing in the field of consciousness studies is the awareness of the multi-dimensionality of consciousness.
I'm inclined to think that though social constructionism has done a good service in making the social sciences more aware of the influence of social factors in the way we “construct” reality, it has not helped in this area. The different viewpoints social constructionism deals with are still differences within the OWS, and as such it still describes variations within the Flatlanders' world.
Multiple truths? Criticising
is not much use unless one has something better to suggest, and one
may doubt that the Indian system of which I have presented some basic
methodologies fares any better than the one psychology uses at
present. The most common objection is that the various spiritual and
religious systems that have developed within India seem to have come
up with entirely different maps of the subtle worlds they all claim
to describe. Interestingly, the Indian tradition itself has no
difficulty with this. At least 2500 years before Katz, the Taittiriya
Upanishad (2.6) cheerfully declares: “Whoever envisages it as the
existence, becomes that existence, and whoever envisages it as the
non-existence, becomes that non-existence”. Interestingly, this was
not taken as a sign that both parties where imagining things. On the
contrary, it was taken as a sign that the ultimate reality goes
beyond our mental differentiations. One of India's most well-known mahavakya,
“great words”, says,
“Truth is one while the wise give it many names”. The ancient
phrase does not ridicule the many different ways the truth is seen:
it is not blind men but “the wise” who use different names. The
logic behind is that the ultimate reality is ineffable, or, in the
much nicer Sanskrit expression, anantaguna,
of infinite quality, while our minds are too small to grasp that
infinity. So to express the One whom we can become or know in a deep
inner sense but not “understand” in a superficial mental way, all
we can do is describe some “pen-ultimate” experience, which
points to an indescribable way beyond itself. Consistent with the
idea that to get at the truth one should be detached from one's own
thinking, ideas, philosophies and even the most sublime experiences
are used as tools to get in contact with a reality that surpasses
them. While fully acknowledging that there is only one ultimate
Truth, the Indian tradition sees no point in trying to arrive at a
single universal formulation of that Truth.7 This deep respect for variety is one of the great achievements of the
Indian tradition, which humanity might well take note of if we want
to keep our conflict ridden multicultural world together. If there is any
truth in all this, then the fact that there are such big differences
between the various spiritual traditions does not have any decisive
bearing on the question whether spiritual experiences are in the end
only brain-states or genuine pointers to a non-physical, but
nevertheless objective, shared reality that truly exists “out
there”. For the individual this question can be settled in many
different ways amongst which personal experience probably plays the
biggest role. For the collective, this question will probably be
settled in due time by accumulating evidence of the existence or
absence of shared experiences and perhaps especially of incidents
where people can accurately observe what other people do in shared
inner spaces. It looks to me that such evidence exists aplenty even
now, both in the spiritual and the parapsychological literature. Of
these two, the former is rich in content but vulnerable to the
objection that some of it might be due to conscious or subconscious
deception. The latter body of literature is comparatively simple in
content and structure but also relatively solid: academic
parapsychology has probably done more to arrive at fraud- and
deception-proof evidence than any other branch of science. The fact
that after more than hundred years of such “fraud-proof” research
there is still a group of vociferous sceptics seems to show that, at
least for some people, personal experience (or the lack of it) plays
a more decisive role than objective research in shaping their
opinion. But in the end, answering these questions may not be as important as it seems. Interestingly, not knowing whether these differences are differences of fact or interpretation does not stand in the way of developing a good science of inner exploration that consists both of generic rules and methods, and of site-specific knowledge and advice. It may be too early to arrive at definite conclusions about the exact geography of the inner realms, but we may have reached the stage where we can at least begin to develop the methods that will enable us to explore the territory effectively. It would be utter folly if for this highly important enterprise the newly arising global civilization would ignore the treasures already collected in Asia.
ReferencesAurobindo, Sri (1995). “The Doctrine of the Mystics” in Aurobindo (1995); accessed on 20-12-2006 at http://www.saccs.org.in/texts/sriaurobindo/sa-doctrineofthemystics.html. —— (1995), The Secret of the Veda, Pondicherry: Sri Aurobindo Ashram. Cornelissen, Matthijs (2001),Introducing Indian
Psychology: the Basics, paper presented at
the National Seminar on Psychology in India, Kollam, Kerala; accessed
on 20-12-2006 at Dalal. A.S. (2001) “Reversal of consciousness: Thoughts on the psychology of the new birth” in Matthijs Cornelissen (Ed.) Consciousnesness and Its transformation. Pondicherry: SAICE. Forman, Robert K. C. ed. (1990).The Problem of Pure Consciousness, Oxford: Oxford University Press. Huxley, Aldous (1946). The Perennial Philosophy, London: Chatto and Windus. Jung, C. G., 1958, Psychology and Religion: West and East, Collected Works, Vol. XI, Bollingen Series XX, Pantheon Books. Katz, Steven T. (1978), “Language, Epistemology and Mysticism,” in Steven T. Katz ed., Mysticism and Philosophical Analysis, pp. 62-63, New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Murphy, Michael and Donovan, Steven (1997), The Physical and Psychological Effects of Meditation, Sausalito, CA: Institute of Noetic Sciences. Skinner, Quentin (1985), Introduction in The Return of Grand Theory in the Human Sciences, Quentin Skinner (Ed.), Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Tart, C. (1972). States of consciousness and state-specific sciences. Science, 176, 1203-1210.
1 This happened at the 2001 annual conference of the Consciousness and Experimental Psychology Section of the British Psychological Society in Oxford. Everybody present nodded in solemn agreement. 2 Nasruddin tells the story of a man who is searching under a street light for keys he has lost inside his house. When questioned the man replies, “What to do? Inside it is dark!” 3 The Rig Veda is from a very early period of human history and its language and symbolism are open to different, even conflicting, interpretations. Traditionally the symbolism is understood to allow interpretation at three levels: the most outward, Ādhibhautika, where it is appears to deal with the technicalities of various sacrifices; an intermediate level, Ādhidaivata, where the various godheads and forces of nature are involved; and a deep inner level, Ādhyātmika, that deals with subtle spiritual processes, powers and realisations. Most non-Indian and even many Indian academics have limited themselves to the first two levels, though it is only at the third level that the text becomes fully understandable and internally coherent. It is also only an interpretation at this level that explains the high esteem in which this text has been held throughout Indian history. For a short introduction, see “The Doctrine of the Mystics” and for a further exposition, The Secret of the Veda (both: Aurobindo, 1995). 4 The Indian tradition asserts that if one does so, one discovers that in the end all consciousness is one, so that if one goes deep enough inside oneself, one can also access the consciousness of others and even the subtle knowledge structures that underlie reality itself. This seems a bold claim, but one could argue that in Western science something akin to the latter is done with mathematics. 5 “To us, consciousness is inconceivable without an ego…. If there is no ego there is nobody to be conscious of anything. The ego is therefore indispensable to the conscious process….an ego-less mental condition can only be unconscious to us, for the simple reason that there would be nobody to witness it…. I cannot imagine a conscious mental state that does not relate to a subject, that it, to an ego.” (Carl G. Jung, 1958, p. 484, quoted in Dalal, 2001) 6 In his little masterpiece, “The Problem of Pure Consciousness” (1990), Forman has made an excellent case for the possibility of an entirely “pure consciousness” and he seems to have refuted effectively the various arguments Katz (1972) and others have brought in to argue the opposite. 7 This is not to say that there have not been fierce philosophical disputes during India's long history, especially during the middle ages, but one does not find them in the older scriptures, like the Vedas and the older Upanishads. In modern folktales acrimonious debates are systematically scoffed at: uneducated but pure at heart little girls typically outwit pedantic and arrogant scholars. |