Indian thought and tradition: A psychohistorical perspective
S. K. Kiran Kumar
Psychology as a scientific discipline has a short history of hundred and odd years but it has a long past as a subject of human inquiry. In all cultures we find some psychological concepts and theories employed by people. Different labels like naïve psychology, commonsense psychology, ethnopsychology, everyday psychology, and the psychology of the populace are used to refer to them (Thomas, 2001). The agenda of modern scientific psychology is to move from these naïve and commonsense notions towards a universally applicable set of concepts, principles and theories. In the jargon of cross-cultural psychology, the attempt is to move from “emic” to “etic” or from “culture specific” to “culture free” knowledge. Unfortunately, however, the scientific psychology that emerged in the West has itself turned out to be another version of emic or culture-specific or ethnopsychology, being heavily influenced by the cultural values and outlook of the Western world (Tart, 1975a; Kim and Berry, 1993; Much, 1995). Its claim of universality is being viewed as “imposed etic” rather than “derived etic” (Kim and Berry, 1993). Modern scientific psychology fails to account for all the variations of human behavior across cultures and it is also limited in its scope and leaves out all those aspects of human nature and experience that do not fit the framework of scientific approach. It is in this scenario that psychologists all over the world have felt the need to examine the “indigenous psychologies” (Kim and Berry, 1993; Kim and Park, 2005) for their contemporary relevance to account for behavior, locally and globally, wherever they have the potential utility. While there are some who hold that psychology by its very nature cannot claim to be culture free and universal laws and principles cannot be arrived at, there are others who strongly believe in such a possibility. This has resulted in what is known as contextualism-universalism debate (Bem and Looren de Jong, 1997; Capaldi and Proctor, 1999).
It is interesting and instructive to note that Wilhelm Wundt, who first established the experimental psychology laboratory, also devoted himself to examine the sociocultural influences in psychological processes and authored a ten-volume work on Volkerpsychologie. For him, Volkerpsychologie (folk psychology) concerned “those mental products created by a community of human life and are therefore inexplicable in terms merely of individual consciousness, since they presuppose the reciprocal action of many” (Wundt, 1912/1916, p.3, cited in Thomas, 2001, p.1). Thomas has used the term folk psychology, in keeping with Wundt’s definition, to mean “a collection of beliefs, shared by members of a cultural group, regarding how people think when they interpret life’s events” (2001, p.1). They include events that are physical, biological, economic, social, personal, and more. For instance, Heimann (1964) in her Facets of Indian thought notes that all aspects of Indian tradition are governed by a transcendental world-view.
According to Thomas, folk psychology can be understood with reference to a pair of questions. (a) By what thought processes do the members of a cultural group account for the events of the world - including their own and other people’s behavior? (b) What beliefs (Wundt’s “mental products”) do people acquire by means of those processes? Wundt’s and Thomas’s conception of folk psychology involves people’s notions about how humans think and also the products of mind that result from such thinking. The latter includes what is otherwise known as indigenous knowledge. It is defined as people’s “cognitive wise legacy as a result of their interaction with nature in a common territory” (Maurial, 1999, p.62 - cited in Thomas, 2001, p.7). It is also defined as the “knowledge that has evolved in a particular societal context and which is used by lay people in that context of life in the conduct of their lives” (George, 1999, p.80, cited in Thomas, p. 7). Such indigenous knowledge prevalent in different cultures and societies leads to diversity of viewpoints on reality, knowing, cause, competence, innate characteristics, attributes of self, esteemed traits, consequence of behavior, values, prohibitions, life versus death, human versus nonhuman, happiness and sadness, normal versus abnormal, family structure and function, social class, justice, ethnicity, work and play,and so on (Thomas, 2001). All these taken together constitute folk psychology and it means something more than just theories of mind and behavior prevalent in different cultures.
Many contemporary researchers who are using the phrase indigenous psychology often limit its meaning and scope to indigenous theories of mind and behavior divorcing it from the “mental products” that result. Thomas is convinced that understanding of folk psychology is delimited by this. He argues that a group’s beliefs serve as “valuable lenses through which to view the member’s shared thought process” (ibid, p.4). That is, interpretations of how people think--people’s mental functions--are typically inferences drawn from those individuals’ expressed beliefs and displayed behaviors and hence indigenous psychology needs to be defined in this inclusive sense, synonymous with folk psychology. In this chapter I will use the term indigenous psychology with this broader connotation. In Indian tradition, the four Vedas; 108 principal Upaniṣads; the six orthodox systems--Pūrva-Mīmāṃsa, Uttara Mīmāṃsa (Vedānta), Sāṁkhya, Yoga, Nyāya, and Vaiśeṣika; the two heterodox systems -- Jainism and Buddhism; Dharma Śāstra, Gṛhya Sūtras, Purāṇas, Rāmāyaṇa, Mahābhārata, Āyurveda samhitās and others are the repertoire of indigenous knowledge and constitute indigenous psychologies. These are the Wundt’s “mental products”, created by our ancient seers and sages, which serve as valuable lenses to understand the Indian psyche.
Some Characteristics of Indian Psyche
India has been a home for many diverse groups of people from all over the world from ancient times. Geographically, in ancient times, India was known as Bhārata khanda stretching extensively in the North, North-East and North-West. It was a continent (khanda in Sanskrit). People from all parts of the world settled in Bhārata. For example, there were “the Eolithic Negroids from Africa; the Proto-Australoids and the Austric peoples, probably from Western Asia; the Mongoloids from the Far East in their various ramifications; the congeries of the Asian peoples who appear to have brought the Dravidian language and culture into India; the Indo-Europeans in their various elements, racial and linguistic--not only Nordic, but also Mediterranean, Alpine, and Dimaric in race as well as language; Aryan--both as Indo-Aryan and Iranian--as well as Proto-Hellenic and historical Hellenic; and other races and peoples, too numerous to mention even for the prehistoric period only1” (Chatterji, Pusalker and Dutt, 1958, p. viii - xi).
In addition, in the long history many other people arrived, including “the Assyrio Babylonians, the ancient Persians, the Greeks, the Scythians, the Parthians and other Iranians, the Turks, the Muslim Persians, the Armenians, and the modern West European peoples like the Portuguese, the French, the Dutch and the English; possibly a backwash of Polynesians, besides, Elamites, Finno-Ugrians and some others” (Chatterji, Pusalker and Dutt, 1958, p. xi). It appears Bhārata was a colorful mosaic a few thousand years ago with all these different races living in one land and they jointly built up the culture we associate with India. They built up “the Wonder that is India” (Chatterji, Pusalker and Dutt, 1958, p. xxxix) and succeeded in flavoring the unique picture of multiculturalism. Hence, it is wrong to treat Indian psyche as a homogenous entity from a cultural point of view. We have “multicultural minds” (Hong, Morris, Chiu, Benet-Martinez, 2000) in India and we have indigenous psychologies!
It is such diversity with some sort of unity that has baffled many scholars, foreign and Indian, who have attempted to understand the uniqueness of India. Ramanujan (1990) has raised a fundamental question – “Is there an Indian way of thinking?” This question is very important from the point of view of developing Indian psychology. He has parsed the question in four different ways: Is there an Indian way of thinking? Is there an Indian way of thinking? Is there an Indian way of thinking? Is there an Indian way of thinking? Each of them laid emphasis on a particular issue. His answers to these questions reveal how difficult it is to understand and develop Indian psychology and make us wonder about the reality of such an enterprise.
Ramanujan’s first question, “Is there an Indian way of thinking?” raises the most fundamental issue. If there is nothing like that, all this talk of Indian psychology is irrelevant and useless. Ramanujan says there was an Indian way of thinking and it is not so any more. Therefore, if one wants to learn about it, he says “do not ask modern--day citified Indians”, instead, go to the pundits, the vaidyas, and the old texts. However, in the next breath he asserts, “India never changes; under the veneer of the modern, Indians still think like the Vedas” (p. 41). Ramanujan’s answers appear paradoxical, yet on serious consideration, we can very clearly see that though forces of modernization and Westernization have brought substantial changes in our country, we have still retained many characteristics of the past that may be called Indian and we necessarily have to go back in time if we want to develop that. However, as he points out in relation to his second question, there is no single Indian way of thinking. We have great and little traditions; ancient and modern, rural and urban, classical and folk. Each language, caste and region has its special worldview. This substantiates what I have said earlier, that we have indigenous psychologies. Yet we can think of Indian Psychology because, “under the apparent diversity, there is a reality, a unity of view point, a single super system...” (ibid. p 41-42). What is that, which can be called Indian? This brings us to Ramanujan’s third question, “Is there an Indian way of thinking?” According to him, this question can be answered in both ways. He states from an onlooker’s viewpoint, there is nothing special to India. It can be characterized as nothing but pre-industrial, pre-printing press, face-to-face, agricultural, feudal, etc. Marxists, Freudians, McLuhanites, and other theorists according to their scheme of social evolution have their tables and can fit India in one of their stages. India is only an example of their scheme. Others, however, would argue about the uniqueness of the Indian way and how it turns all things, especially rivals and enemies into itself. They assert there is an Indian way, and “it imprints and patterns all things that enter the continent; it is inescapable, and it is Bigger Than All of Us” (p. 42).
Lannoy (1971), for instance, has highlighted three features of Indian thought that have demonstrated ‘survival value’ over a long stretch of time, which originated in remote antiquity. They are: the absolute authority of the Word as universal metaphysical principle; the concept of the ‘flux of all things’; and hylozoistic thinking2. According to him the outstanding characteristic of a primitive society is its sense of the unity of all life, the sense of solidarity and emotional sympathy. This is reflected in the primitive tendency to draw no clear distinction between matter, life and mind, which he terms as hylozoistic thinking. He notes that this idea is encountered in the theory of guṇas3 (sattva, rajas, and tamas), which is the common stuff of both mind and matter. Lannoy hastens to add that this type of thinking should not be misconstrued as some form of ‘pre-logical irrationalism’. On the other hand, man as a microcosmic organism under primitive, pre-technological conditions, sees himself as an exact counterpart of the macrocosm-Nature and hence obtains knowledge of the latter in a direct way, by immediate sensory awareness. Thus both pre-Socratic Greek thought and Indian thought started from the assumption that without an identity between the knowing subject and the reality known, the fact of knowledge would be unaccountable. Referring specifically to Indian thought, Lannoy notes that for Indians, subject and object, thesis and antithesis, are not opposed to one another, but constitute two aspects of the same thing and all differentiation springs from the coincidentia oppositorum of a unitary Absolute. Lannoy further adds that the identity between the knower and the known was not just an assumption, “but was a mode of perception”. He also clarifies that there is nothing ‘mystical’ or ‘uncanny’ in this kind of thought if one uses the word mystical in the sense which Santayana gives it - the most natural mode of thinking. It was suppressed in Western thinking for many centuries through specialization in a certain kind of consciousness. While Lannoy’s understanding of Indian thought seems accurate, the question to be asked here is whether the mode of perception he refers to can be called primitive since that term has a pejorative connotation in modern context.
Swami Ajaya (1983), a disciple of Swami Rama of the Himalayan International Institute of Yoga, has noted that there are two kinds of opposites--“antithetical” and “complementary”. He proposes that complementarity of opposites is a characteristic of what he calls “dualistic” thought which is a stage beyond Piaget’s formal-operational thought. An Indian example of this thought is Sāṁkhya and Western examples are Jung’s theory and Systems theory. Beyond this dualistic thought there is “unitive thought”. Advaita Vedānta is an example of this stage. In support of his propositions he cites the work of two developmental psychologists, Herb Koplowitz and Klaus Regal, who have postulated these two stages of development beyond Piaget’s four stages. Ajaya hastens to add that unitive thought is different from “unitive consciousness” also known as “pure consciousness” or Brahman/Ātman in Indian tradition. Ajaya’s hypothesis indicates that the psychological foundation of Indian tradition is qualitatively distinct from that of Western thought and also possibly of a higher stage in intellectual development. In view of this it may be detrimental to the cause of Indian thought to understand it from one of those readymade Western theories available to us. Many cross-cultural psychology and social psychology researches use one of those Western generated templates of social evolution to characterize Indian society and to describe the behavior of Indians. Characterizing India as a collectivistic society is an example of this, though Indian researchers have found that both individualism and collectivism trends are present in our culture (Sinha and Tripathi, 1994). Similarly, Kakar’s (1996) Indian Psyche from Freudian perspective is another instance of such an approach. While such an approach can throw useful light, it can also distort reality to some extent.
The task at hand for us is to identify what is that Indian way, rather than making use of one of those Western templates readily available to fit the Indian psyche to that. If we have to do that we have to take Ramanujan’s fourth question seriously, i.e., Is there an Indian way of thinking? Referring to this, he observes that this may mean whether Indians think at all, because there is an opinion developed among scholars that it is the West that is materialistic and rational. On the other hand, Indians have no philosophy, only religion; no positive sciences, not even a psychology; in India matter is subordinated to spirit, rational thought to feeling and intuition. Ramanujan observes, even when people agree that this is the case, we can have arguments for and against it. “Some lament, others celebrate Indian’s un-thinking ways” (1990, p. 42). The implication is that the thought pattern of Indians is distinctly different from that of others and we have discussed it in the previous paragraphs. What is important for us is to decide whether lay people and seers and sages shared a similar thought pattern or whether a distinctive thought pattern was characteristic of the latter.
In his attempts to find an answer to this question Ramanujan approaches the subject from a linguistic view point and applies grammatical rules as a tool for this purpose. He refers to the two of them, viz., “context-free” and “context-sensitive”. An example of context-free rule is “sentence must have subjects and predicates in a certain relation”. An example of context-sensitive rule is “plurals in English are realized as –s after stops (dog-s, cat-s), -es before fricatives (e.g., latch-es), -ren after the word child, etc.” (p. 47). Extending these rules to cultures, Ramanujan observes that cultures have a tendency to idealize and think in terms of either one of them and people are guided by them in their behavior. According to him, in cultures like India the context-sensitive kind of rule is the preferred formulation and it is evident in almost all aspects of Indian life.
Ramanujan (1990) attributes it to the Hindu concern with jāti – the logic of classes, of genera and species, of which human jātis are only an instance. In his view, “each jāti or class defines a context, a structure of relevance, a rule of permissible combinations, a frame of reference, and a meta-communication of what is and can be done” (p. 53). In his opinion the various taxonomies of season, landscape, times, qualities (guṇas) and their material bases, tastes, characters, emotions, etc., provide the context and are basic to the thought work of Hindu medicine and poetry, cooking and religion, erotic and magic. However, he notes that societies have “underbellies” and if one rule is predominant the other will be the dominant ideal and surfaces in the form of counter movements. In his opinion, in traditional cultures like India, where context-sensitivity rules and binds, the dream is to be free of context. That is the ideal and it expresses in different aspects of life.
Rasa in aesthetics, mokṣa in the ‘aims of life’, sanyāsa in the life stages, and sphoṭa in semantics and bhakti in religion are examples in Indian culture. They define themselves against a background of inexorable contextuality. The ancient seers, sages, and thinkers of India seem to have developed a concept and system of life that structured around a movement towards the context-free rule from context-sensitivity as recognizable in these domains. Referring to puruṣārthas (goals of life) he states that karma, artha, and dharma are all relational in their values, tied to place, time, personal character and social role, but mokṣa is the release from all relations. Similarly, referring to four āśramas he states that while the first two, brahmacārya (celibate studentship) and gṛhastha (householder stage) are preparation and fulfillment of a relational life, vānaprastha (the retiring forest-dweller stage) and sanyāsa (renunciation) loosens the bond and cremates all one’s past and present relations. In the realm of feeling, rasa is generalized, it is an essence. On the other hand, bhāvas are private, contingent, context roused sentiments, vibhāvas are determinant causes, anubhāvas, the consequent expressions. In the field of meaning, sphoṭa, an explosion, a meaning which is beyond sequence and time is the culmination of the temporal sequence of letters and phonemes, the syntactic chain of words. “In each of these the pattern is the same: a necessary sequence in time with strict rules of phase and context ending in a free state” (ibid. p. 54). However, bhakti,according to him, is an exception and it denies the very need for context. “Bhakti defies all contextual structures: every pigeonhole of caste, ritual, gender, appropriate clothing and custom, stage of life, the whole system of homo hierarchicus (everything in its place) is the target of its irony” (ibid. p. 54). This is the transcendent orientation of Indian culture which Heimann (1964) sees in all its aspects.
Context-sensitive and context-free rule can be meaningfully employed even in understanding certain complex concepts of Indian tradition. In the discussion on the states of consciousness, Advaita Vedānta posits that the ‘reality’ of phenomenal experiences of jāgrat (waking) and swapna (dream) are relative to those contexts and what is context-free is turīyā, which can be approximately translated in English as “pure consciousness”. Therefore, Advaita Vedānta treats waking and dream states at par. Tart (1972) proposed the concept of “state-specific sciences”, which seem to be modern psychology equivalent of the context-sensitive notion, and James (1902/1968) spoke about types of consciousness and corresponding realities. It should be emphasized here that it is the ‘experience’ of turīyā (pure consciousness) by ancient seers and sages (and also modern ones) that formed the fulcrum of what we may call Indian psychology, from which perspective only the relative validity of all human experiences in different states can be appreciated.
Some of these features that characterize Indian thought suggest that all the theories and models of modern psychology cannot explain all the behavior of Indian masses and we need to look within for indigenous knowledge to develop a psychology of our own. On the other hand, some of the features like hylozoistic thinking and an urge towards context-freeness appear to be present universally among peoples of other cultures and they find Indian culture and its thought personally meaningful for them. Many foreigners who got interested in spirituality and dedicated themselves for it serve as the best example of this. What it means is that the psychology that we propose to develop from within is not only useful for our people, it also has psychological significance for persons from other cultures and hence the potentiality to be universal, not just contextual. Therefore, Indian Psychology that is being discussed here is a value addition to the existing psychology expanding its scope and application rather than a regressive movement.
Indian Psychology – a myth or reality?
Though this handbook is aimed at Indian psychology the above question is relevant and valid and needs to be addressed in order to make certain things clear. As noted earlier, we have many indigenous psychologies in India, because of the great diversity of ethnic groups who settled here. If we take a close look at our intellectual legacy it is surprising that in India we do not have a well-known classic on psychology as we can find for other branches of knowledge. For instance, we have Āyurveda as a branch of knowledge and Caraka and Suśrutha samhitās as āyurvedic texts deal with medicine, surgery and longevity. We have Sāṁkhya Kārikā of Īśvara Kṛṣṇa and a number of other texts and treatises that deal with the Sāṁkhya system of philosophy. We have Pāṇini’s Aśtadhyāyi which is an epoch making work on Sanskrit language and linguistics. We also have Vātsayāna’s Kāma Sūtra, related to human sexuality. However, we do not have one such well-known text that directly addresses the nature and functions of mind. The celebrated Yoga-Sūtras of Patañjali though deal with the subject to some extent, it is a practical manual for transcending the functions of mind and hence it is not considered as a text on psychology proper in the modern sense.
Regarding this glaring omission in Indian tradition, S. K. R. Rao (1962) observes that in India, our ancient thinkers did not give psychology an independent educational status as a branch of knowledge, nor do we find an exact word for psychology in Sanskrit. They did not consider it worth pursuing as an independent subject apart from their spiritual pursuits and discouraged others from doing so, since they perceived too much preoccupation with mind and its function as a hindrance in achieving transcendence. This point is made by Chennakesavan (1980) as follows: “the purpose of the Upaniṣads is to reveal Brahman, the supreme self, and a distinct warning is sounded to the seekers of truth, not to be carried away by manas and its attributes, but to try to know the thinker (mantr). It is stressed often that Prajñā which is the self or Ātman is responsible for the activities of manas…Mind or manas, therefore, plays only a secondary role in knowledge.”
Nevertheless, as Rao pointed out, there was enough psychologising and our ancients in their attempts to solve the philosophical problems necessarily addressed psychological topics such as consciousness, experience, perception, illusion, will, desires, emotions and others. Enquiries concerning those psychological processes did not occur in a single context, or as a connected account. The field of philosophical enquiry was structured into numerous interests – orthodox and heterodox – and the compilation of relevant reflections of all of them concerning a specific problem was almost impossible. Further, Indian works are written in multiplicity of languages. Vedas are in archaic Sanskrit; the Upaniṣads and later scholastic works are in classical Sanskrit; the early Buddhist texts are in Pāli; and the Jaina texts are in Ardhamagadhi and mixed-Sanskrit. A competent presentation of Indian psychological thought requires “knowledge of Sanskrit, Pāli, and Ardhamagadhi (all three languages are almost dead now); acquaintance with Indian philosophy and allied branches of study; and academic training in modern psychology” (Rao, 1962, p. vii), which is a rare and unusual combination to think of4.
Further, psychological speculations existed in diverse and even disparate disciplines like metaphysics and medicine, logic and sexology, and religion and poetry. No writer could be expected to have enough mastery over all of them to sieve out the psychological contributions (Rao, 1962). No ancient thinker seems to have attempted to integrate and systematize them and hence we do not find any classical text or treatise exclusively devoted to this subject. In other words, for ancient seers and sages delineation and discussion of mind and its functions was like describing the many places visited by a traveler in his travelogue, whose destination is something far away from all those places. Therefore, we do not find readily available models, theories, and systems of Indian psychology. So in creating a body of knowledge called Indian psychology, out of diverse sources, we face many problems like lack of clarity, lack of details, lack of compatibility, lack of continuity and such others, of ideas and views. Given this type of a situation, contemporary efforts at carving out Indian psychology appear both challenging as well as perplexing.
In contrast to this situation is the initiative of some Western scholars a century ago in bringing together the scattered ideas of psychological nature of Indian tradition, which laid the foundation for Indian Psychology. Though the term Indian psychology was used explicitly by Sri Aurobindo in one of his writings on education in 1910 (Chakraborthy, 1991), perhaps the first published work on this subject is that of Mrs. Rhys Davids, titled The Birth of Indian Psychology and its development in Buddhism in 1936. Prior to this, Davids published Buddhist Psychology in 1914, but no known published work seems to have existed explicitly with a title on Indian psychology before her work. However, there were other publications also in the West of a comparative nature between modern psychology and Indian psychological thought. That include, Western Psychotherapy and Hindu Sādhanā (Jacobs, 1901) and Yoga and Western Psychology: A comparison (Coster, 1934). Following this some Indians also authored a few books and they were published in the West. Most notable among them are Indian Psychology: Perception (Sinha, 1936), Hindu Psychology: It’s Meaning for the West and Mental Health and Hindu Psychology (Swami Akhilananda, 1948, 1952). Such initial attempts stimulated others to work in this direction.
Though Western scholars were aware of the value of psychological insights available in Indian tradition a century ago, interest in them did not percolate into the work of academicians in India barring few exceptions. Durganand Sinha (1993) who advocated indigenization of psychology in India, initially expressed reservations against the idea of Indian psychology on the grounds that scientific laws and principles are of a universal character and they cannot be delimited by geographical boundaries. He also pointed out that the early attempts at formulation of Indian psychology were rejected by empirically trained psychologists in India, on the grounds that the notions of karma, reincarnation, Self-realization and others, which are central to Indian tradition, are unscientific. This is an instance of rejecting the mental products and the thought processes that are integral for an indigenous psychology as defined in the beginning. Further, Krishnan (1994) points out that majority of the psychologists have preferred to work in the mainstream since the traditional ideas may not help in academic progress, in gaining recognition, and in securing grants for research projects. Adair et al. (1995, p. 393) have found that academic psychologists in India expressed significantly greater need for indigenization, but they did not endorse substantially the idea that psychology can be made more relevant to the local context by a “return to Vedic or other traditional writings”. As a result, though many psychologists in India in mid 1960s and in 1970s recognized the need for making psychology relevant to their sensibility, it largely remained a-cultural (Pandey, 2001). These reasons have obscured the value and significance of Indian psychological thought and they have remained isolated without being integrated into the academic pursuits of psychologists in India.
A question that may arise is why bother about those ideas, which our ancestors themselves did not consider worthy of independent pursuit. Further, one may also wonder in what way are those ancient speculations relevant in this post-modern context, either for India or for a universal body of knowledge. To answer these questions we need to examine the contemporary status of scientific psychology.
Ever since its inception, scientific psychology progressively veered away from the psyche in its original sense (soul) as the subject matter of study. The definition of psychology underwent changes replacing its subject matter from consciousness to mind and then to behavior. This movement reflects the need felt by those pioneers in the field to make psychology rigorously scientific. As a result, much of what rightfully belongs to human experience was relegated to the background as non-scientific, as “not me”, resulting in what may be termed here as the subconscious region of psychology, in the Freudian sense. For Behaviorists it is the unconscious region! Whatever that is termed as mainstream scientific psychology, with which many psychologists identify as “me”, can be called the conscious region of psychology by analogy. In other words, through the behaviorist era, many concepts like consciousness, dreams, free-will, extrasensory perception, intuition, wisdom, transcendence and others were not considered as valid subject matter and often one does not find them in textbooks of introductory psychology. Even if they are, their discussion revolves around certain limited aspects related to what is accepted as scientific. For instance, discussion on dreams involves the physiological correlates rather than the phenomenology of dreams. Consciousness is discussed in terms of reticular formation and higher cortical processes. Similarly love is discussed in terms of its underlying glandular basis or cerebral correlates. This exclusionist and reductionistic approach in psychology has been a point of dispute ever since its inception and its adequacy and validity as an appropriate paradigm for psychology are being increasingly questioned in recent years (Bem and Loorie de Jong, 1997). A conflict has always been there between the subconscious and the conscious regions of psychology.
A consequence of this situation is the emergence, over the past several decades, of many psychologies and perspectives that address all those ideas, concepts and approaches that constitute what I have termed as the subconscious region of psychology. These include such major movements like humanistic and transpersonal psychology (Maslow, 1971), and also such specific schools and/or approaches like indigenous psychology (Kim and Berry, 1993); phenomenological psychology, symbolic interactionism, social representations, cultural psychology, feminism and psychology, discursive psychology, dialogical psychology, narratology, ideography and the case study (Smith, Harre, Van Langenhove, 1995, 1995a). It is to be emphasized that all these have emerged in the Western context itself alongside mainstream scientific psychology and hence amply indicate that at present, psychology as a discipline is multivocal. Thus, in contemporary psychology we can identify two distinct pursuits of interest viz., mainstream scientific psychology that is dominant and adhered to by majority and alternative schools, systems, perspectives and movements spearheaded by either individual or cohorts of psychologists, which differ either in focus or in approach or in both from the mainstream.
Among these, two specific developments in the discipline have played key roles in stimulating investigators to examine and incorporate Indian psychological thought into current literature. First, in 1960s and 70s, the increasing popularity of meditation and Yoga (Naranjo and Ornstein, 1971), the increasing interest in the study of consciousness (Ornstein, 1972, 1973; Tart, 1969; 1975b) and the emergence of transpersonal psychology (Tart, 1975a) brought to focus the Indian religio-spiritual and philosophical traditions as valuable resources to understand the nature of human mind and consciousness. Psychologists in the West found that these sources can provide theoretical and practical knowledge on exploring human psyche beyond the range of ordinary awareness and also fill the void in modern psychology regarding the understanding of self (Tart, 1975a). In recognition of this, Tart (1975a) considered them “transpersonal psychologies” and Walsh (1980) termed them as “consciousness disciplines”. With this many investigators have taken interest in studying Vedas, Upaniṣads, Yoga-Sūtras, Bhagavad Gītā and other related literature not as holy books, but as psychological documents of universal significance. Many Indian concepts have found their way into academic discussions and writings in the West.
Second, the increase in cross-cultural research and the development of cross-cultural psychology (Berry, Poortinga, Dasen and Seagal, 1992) led to greater appreciation of the paradigmatic limitations of modern scientific psychology, which render the claim of its universality doubtful. A common refrain is that the so-called universal laws and principles of modern psychology are nothing but one type of ‘indigenous psychology’, developed within the Euro-American ethos determined by the underlying worldview of the West and hence cannot be generalized to other cultures (Kim and Berry, 1993). Many psychologists have been arguing that culture and psyche are integrally related and one cannot be understood without the other (Gergen, Gulerce, Lock, and Misra, 1996), and more and more psychologists – western and non-western – emphasized on understanding human behavior within a cultural context and on the necessity of developing local or indigenous theories and perspectives (Kim and Park, 2005). It has been expressed differently by investigators viz., “ethnopsychology”, “cultural psychology” and “folk psychology” (Thomas, 2001). Some researchers in India have been pursuing their interest from this perspective. Thus, contemporary interest in ancient Indian thought and psychological speculations available in them have to be understood in this broader context, because it is not just a matter of nationalism or patriotism of a few Indian psychologists.
In view of the contemporary usage of the phrase “Indian psychology” by many scholars in different contexts, it can be interpreted in five ways. (1) Psychology in India; (2) Indian psychological thought; (3) psychology with an Indian identity; (4) a psychology created by Indian psychologists; and (5) psychology of Indian people (Kumar, 2001).
Psychology in India refers to the history and current status of psychology as an academic discipline or a professional specialty within the country (see A. K. Dalal, 2002; Misra and Mohanty, 2002).
Indian psychological thought represents the psychological insights available in different orthodox systems like Vedānta, Sāṁkhya, Yoga, Nyāya, Vaiśeṣika and heterodox systems like Jainism and Buddhism and other sources. Indian scholars trained in modern psychology like J. Sinha (1958, 1961, 1969/68?), S. K. R Rao, (1962), Safaya (1975), Atreya, (1985), Kuppuswamy (1985), Paranjpe (1984, 1998), Srivastava (2001), K. R. Rao, (2005) and many others have endeavored to collate and present these insights in a manner that is compatible with modern psychological texts.
Psychology with an Indian identity refers to such terms as “Buddhist Psychology” (David, 1916, 1936), “Vedic Psychology” (Dandekar, 1941/1081), “Hindu Psychology” (Akhilananda, 1948, 1952), “Jaina Psychology” (Khalgatgi, 1961; Mehta, 1955), “Psychology of Yoga” (Taimni, 1973), “Dravidian Psychology” (Shaktidharan and Sathya Kumar, 2001), etc.
Psychology created by Indian psychologists refers to a system(s) of psychology developed by Indian psychologists, who adhere to a meta-theoretical or philosophical position. Examples of this are Integral Psychology (Sen, 1986) and A Greater Psychology - An introduction to the psychological thought of Sri Aurobindo (A. S. Dalal, 2001), both derived from Sri Aurobindo’s writings.
Psychology of Indian people refers to a body of psychological knowledge, about patterns of thinking and behaving and to the understanding of the behavior of Indian masses. Researchers mostly working in the area of cross-cultural and social psychology have made some of the major arguments in favor of a cultural approach to the development of such a psychology of Indian people (Krishnan, 2003; Misra, 2000, 2001; Misra, Jain and Singh, 1995; Neki, 2000; D. Sinha, 1997). The contemporary attempts at developing Indian psychology reflect many of these concerns and we find researchers working from different perspectives (see Kumar, in press).
Origins of psychological thought in ancient India
As noted earlier, Bhārata Khanda was vast and many ethnic groups settled in different parts of this continent. The sophisticated Indus Valley Civilization (c. 3300–1700 BC, flourished 2600–1900 BCE), flourished in the Indus and Ghaggar-Hakra river valleys primarily in what is now Pakistan and western India, parts of Afghanistan and Turkmenistan. Another name for this civilization is the Harappan Civilization, after the first of its cities to be excavated, Harappa. The Dravidians of South India are ancient inhabitants of the Indian peninsula, with their distinct language and culture existing to date. In Northern India, waves of invaders made the peninsula their home and added to the cultural fabric of India. The conglomeration of different worldviews and indigenous psychologies over few thousand years seem to have coalesced due to mutual influences of cultures and some kind of synthesis has taken place as reflected in indigenous knowledge related to reality, knowing, cause, life versus death, attributes of self, human versus nonhuman, happiness and sadness, consequence of behavior, values, justice, prohibitions, social class, family structure and function and others. The differences between them have not disappeared, but exist alongside and developed into the different orthodox and heterodox Indian systems. The orthodox is so called because all the thinkers in those schools owe their allegiance to Vedas as authority, whilst the heterodox schools do not accept the authority of Vedas. Most scholars refer to the psychological perspectives available in orthodox and heterodox schools as Indian psychology proper. Thus we have two basic streams of thought development in Indian tradition, which may be considered as precursors of Indian psychological thought. However, all these schools have been influenced by the indigenous psychologies of diverse ethnic groups about which we have no knowledge. Jadunath Sinha’s three volumes on Indian Psychology – Cognition, Emotion and Will, and The Epistemology of Perception (1936/1958, 1961, 1969) are monumental compendiums of all the classical ideas scattered in various sources of orthodox and heterodox schools.
It is conventional in Indian tradition to trace all the ideas and concepts related to indigenous knowledge to the Vedas, which were the first literary products of our ancestors, faithfully passed on in a continuous oral tradition without much distortion. Often there is a tendency to identify India with Vedas, Vedic tradition, and Vedic Aryans exclusively. Vedic tradition, in particular, has retained its oral character even today in the age of information technology and digitization. It has also spread to the European and Western countries and there are many in those countries who are able to chant the Vedas exactly the same way as a traditional Brāhmin can do it or even much better (this author was a witness for the chanting of Rūdram and Camakam by lady disciples of Swami Muktananda in California, USA, all wearing T-shirts and Jean pants on a Śivarātri festival day!).
However, as Prof. Dandekar, a reputed Indologist states: “As a matter of fact, a large number of elements in the classical Hindu way of life and thought clearly betray a pre-Vedic non-Aryan origin” and “the Aryan Vedism may be regarded as a grand interlude in the continuity of ancient Indian thought” (1941/1981, p.339). Unfortunately we do not have much information about pre-Vedic people and their culture and hence cannot say anything about their contribution to the development of Indian thought but for some indirect inferences drawn from the remains of Indus-Valley civilization. No accurate historical or archaeological evidence is available to fix the dates of pre-Vedic and Vedic period. S. K. R. Rao (1962) notes that Jainism is perhaps the oldest religion in the country and even in the Indus civilization traces of Jaina practices like nudity, asceticism, bull-worship, etc., are discernible and it was probably pre-Aryan. Kalghatgi (1961) has noted that Jacobi (1946) traces Jainism to early primitive currents of metaphysical speculations and according to Zimmer (1951), Jainism reflects the cosmology and anthropology of a much older pre-Aryan upper class of North-Eastern India. Hence, from the point of view of the origins of psychological notions, the influence of Jaina worldview and of Indus civilization is no less important than Vedic and post-Vedic period. For the purpose of tracing the development of psychological thought in India it may be essential to consider the influence of pre-Vedic and Vedic traditions.
Since we do not know who were the first dwellers of Indian sub-continent and what was their cognitive structure we cannot speak with certainty on how and when the first psychological notions emerged in India and what thought characteristics distinguished Indians from others. Therefore, to identify what is uniquely Indian that characterizes the multicultural minds and has survived all these years is really a formidable task and we need to consider pre-Vedic, Vedic, and post-Vedic period, as well as the period extending from the time of Mahavīra and Buddha to modern times, spanning almost 5000 years. A problem that we face in this endeavor is that of ancient India’s characteristics, i.e., its a-historical approach to men and matters. This has something to do with the transcendental orientation of Indian tradition already referred to. We do not find either oral or written records of events and personages regarding the time of their occurrences in most cases till Buddha’s period (5th century B.C.E). Therefore, much of the historical work related to ancient India is not very accurate with respect to time. Similarly, regarding the authorships of many ancient works there is uncertainty, because authors often did not care to leave any signs of personal identification. Nevertheless, chronological sequence of events have been maintained and passed on from one generation to the other in continuous oral tradition enabling us to get a sense of what has happened and historians have made attempts to date them depending on the circumstantial and secondary evidences, available in the tradition. Based on such observations scholars have attempted to trace the development of the intellectual legacy of India and their efforts form the base of this essay. Hence, no claims are made to provide an accurate picture of the historical developments of psychological ideas and perspectives. On the other hand, an attempt is made to provide an overview of them.
Muni-Yati and ṚṢi traditions
The variations in ethos (life ways) and eidos (thought ways) of Indians in different regions has been influenced by the psyche of the ancient people who came to India in pre-historic and historic periods. Based on the knowledge of socio-cultural practices prevalent in ancient times Dandekar (1959/1981) distinguishes between “two patterns of life and thought” viz., Non-Aryan, pre-Vedic and Aryan-Vedic. He refers to them as muni-yati tradition and ṛṣi tradition respectively. According to him muni-yati tradition is primarily characterized by (i) prevalence of Siva religion and the practices and cults associated with it; and (ii) glorification of a life of renunciation, asceticism, wandering mendicancy, and severe austerities. It was an “iconic religion” and idol worship was its chief feature. Pūja (rituals) and bali (offerings) were part of this. Some kind of yoga practices, involving “concentration” aimed at “mental control” was another feature (Dandekar, 1959/1981).
In contrast, Rsi tradition of Vedic-people was characterized by (i) homa, offering oblations to god through fire; and (ii) an ideal of a materially secure and prosperous family life. Idol worship was entirely unknown to the Vedic religion (Dandekar, 1959/1981). The Vedic religion did not believe in a personal God. To begin with, it consisted of hymns in praise of various deities who are personifications of the different powers of nature like water, fire, wind, and the like. In addition, there were hymns which describe charms for curing diseases, prayers for long life and health, imprecations against demons, sorcerers and enemies, charms pertaining to women to secure their love, etc. These prayers were recited in the performances of certain prescribed rituals, called yajña (sacrifices) (Dasgupta, 1927). The aim was obtaining material prosperity and happiness. “Sacrificial mysticism” is the term used by Dasgupta (1927) to refer to Vedic Aryan ritualistic religion. He has used the term mysticism and religion interchangeably. In addition, there were other cults like that of Vratyas, the Munis, and the Brahmacārins, which were closely allied to Rūdra-Śiva worship, who followed a fundamentally different life style from that of the Vedic Aryans (Dandekar, 1959/1981).
The muni-yati tradition of pre-Vedic people had spread far and wide and was well established in India. Idol worship, which was the chief characteristic of the religion of pre-Vedic people, implies a concrete representation of some higher force, wherein the signifier and the signified are not of the same order. It represents such distinctions as man–God, worshipper–worshipped, individual self–higher self, etc. The emphasis on a life of renunciation, asceticism, wandering mendicancy, etc., suggests that people were seeking to go beyond the ordinary life and had the notions of transcendence and spirituality. It further suggests that those people had already found a means or a way of transcendence. Faith in such a religion implies that pre-Vedic non Aryan people probably shared a worldview and thought pattern, in which there was a clear demarcation between the personal self and a higher reality. They also had a perspective on life not limited to physical reality. It also suggests that among those people there was a tendency to go inward and away from an active involvement in material life, probably to realize something beyond the mundane existence. Such a tendency, one can speculate, heralded the development of those schools that emphasize on transcendence in one or the other way. It is interesting to note that while Sāṁkhya emphasizes on realizing the puruṣa beyond the prakṛti,Yoga is defined by Patañjali as citta vṛtti nirodha (restriction of the fluctuations of mind). Similarly, Jainism and Buddhism as spiritual movements, emphasize on progressive withdrawal from an active life to attain nirvāṇa. All these represent a dualistic thought.
The emphasis on enjoying material prosperity in the ṛṣi tradition of Vedic people on the other hand, suggests a tendency to go outward and towards active life. It should be noted that the Vedas have many insights of a transcendental nature and hence one need not think that the Vedic seers and sages were interested only in material life. This is exemplified in hymns which say: let one live 100 years in seeing, 100 years in hearing, and so on5. As the oral traditions say, the great seers like Vasiśta, Viśwāmitra, Yāgnavalkya, and others were married, had children and lived full lives.
It is difficult to tell whether the idea of transcendence is of pre-Vedic or Vedic origin. However, the inward and outward emphasis, the introverted and extraverted trait, in the two traditions seems to represent the temperamental difference between the two periods. These two traditions have influenced all the subsequent developments in the Indian sub-continent and psychological notions are no exception to this. But one thing is for sure that, in Indian tradition transcendence as a human possibility has been in the centre stage from ancient times.
The pre-Vedic non-Aryan way of life and thought was so very deeply rooted among large sections of the people that the Vedic-Aryans could not resist its pressure as a popular tradition and were obliged to accept and incorporate many of its features. For instance, the Vedic god Rūdra, is an Aryanised version of the proto-Indian Śiva (Dandekar, 1981, p.337-339). In Dandekar’s opinion the pre-Vedic pattern had extended far and wide and had become firmly established on the Indian soil and influenced the Vedic Aryan ideology in unmistakable terms. Albert Schweitzer (1935) notes that there are two great fundamental problems common to all thought: (1) the problem of world and life affirmation, and world and life negation; and (2) the problem of ethics and the relations between ethics and these two forms of man’s spiritual attitude to Being. World and life affirmation refers to the human tendency “to regard existence as he experiences it in himself and as it has developed in the world as something of value per se and accordingly strives to let it reach perfection in himself, whilst within his own sphere of influence he endeavors to preserve and to further it.” (p. 1). On the other hand, world and life negation refers to the human tendency to regard “existence as he experiences it in himself and as it is developed in the world as something meaningless and sorrowful” and the person “resolves accordingly (a) to bring life to a standstill in himself by mortifying his will-to-live, and (b) to renounce all activity which aims at improvement of the conditions of life in his world” (ibid, p. 1-2). Muni-yati tradition seems to reflect the world and life negation tendency and the Rsi tradition suggests the world and life affirming tendency.
Schweitzer has distinguished these two tendencies from optimism and pessimism. According to him, world and life affirmation and negation are not determined by one’s disposition. It is determined by a person’s inner attitude towards Being, his affirmation or negation of life. It consists in a determination of the will. He notes: “The most profound world and life affirmation is that which has been hard won from an estimate of things unbiased by illusion and even wrested from misfortune, whilst the most profound world and life negation is that which is developed in theory despite of a naturally serene disposition and happy outward circumstances” (ibid, p. 2-3). This point is very important because the Indian orientation to life is often branded as pessimistic.
As to the origin and prevalence of these two orientations in India, Schweitzer notes that it has nothing do with racial differences and observes that the Indian Aryans show an inclination to world and life negation, the Iranian-Persian and the European Aryans to world and life affirmation. He also emphasizes that Indian thought is not exclusively world and life negating and that in the Upaniṣads there is also a certain element of world and life affirmation and it expresses strongly in many writings of Indian literature. He concludes that both of these are present side by side in Indian thought but world and life negation occupies a predominant position. He rejects the possibility of Vedic Aryans being influenced by pre-Vedic- non Aryans in adopting a world and life negating stance. His view is as follows: “The simplest hypothesis is then that the Aryans were originally all followers of world and life affirmation, and that world and life negation originated among the Brāhmins under the influence of the idea of being exalted above this world which was developed from magic-religious ideas and the experience of ecstasy. Only in this way is it comprehensible that the idea of world and life denial in the more ancient period was only represented by the Brāhmins, or to put it better, by certain circles among the Brāhmins, whilst the people held fast to the world and life affirmation which was natural to them. World and life negation belonged to a sacerdotal form of thought which was developed alongside popular thought” (ibid, p. 25-26). Schweitzer’s hypothesis lay emphasis on the primacy of inner experience in transforming one’s orientation to world and life and it appears plausible if we take into account the biography of many sages and saints of the past and present. Most importantly the biography of Sri Ramana Maharshi (see chapter by Anand Paranjpe on Ramana Maharshi) attests this hypothesis. Schweitzer commenting on the role of warrior caste (Kṣatriyas) in developing world and life negating orientation says that: “We do not know how far they made effective the idea of being exalted above the world of the senses to which their thought attained in world and life negation” (ibid, p.26). For this we find an answer in another source.
Barua (1921) has made a significant observation that not only addresses Schweitzer’s doubt but also contradicts a popular belief prevalent in the Indian tradition that world and life negation is Brāhminical. He notes that in the post-Vedic period Brāhmins and Kṣatriyas were advocating two different modes of life and thought. It was the Kṣatriyas who were advocating “the subjective mode of attaining true self-hood and living an ascetic life in the forest, practicing penance and cultivating inner culture and faith” (p. 116). Brāhminic thought, “tended to justify the civic duties of man on the ground of the gradual development of self or gradual manifestation of the potentialities of life…” (p. 116). Ancient seers had anticipated the two growth possibilities for humans, “self-actualization” and “self-transcendence” that Maslow (1971) spoke of.
Taken together the views of Barua, Dandekar and Schweitzer suggest that ancient Indians operated at two levels and two parallel worldviews developed simultaneously, one adopted by lay people and the other by a limited group of persons irrespective of caste. Thomas (2001) has emphasized that a key consideration in recognizing a folk psychology is its group membership. To be considered as a folk psychology a group’s belief system should have adherents from among specialists in knowledge system such as philosophers, priests, shamans, anthropologists, psychiatrists, professional psychologists, medicine men but “most of its members must be ‘common folk’, ones drawn from the lay public” (p.11). He further states that “virtually all religions qualify as folk psychologies, except those few practiced solely by a coterie of specialists (monks, nuns, shamans) who make religion their life’s work and whose beliefs and rituals are not embraced by a lay public” (p.15).
When we examine the Indian scenario and its religious, philosophical and spiritual aspects we find that they are not completely separated and they have been incorporated into the daily life of common folk also. However, both of them have influenced each other mutually and what we have inherited as Indian eidos (thought ways) and ethos (life ways) today, is a ‘compound’ not a ‘mixture’ (in the chemical sense) and hence inseparable. That is why many scholars who are knowledgeable about Hindu tradition are weary of the term Hinduism and prefer to define it as Hindu Way of Life or as sanātana dharma (eternal way of life and values). This can be applied to some extent even for Jainism and Buddhism also. The ancient Indian sages and seers managed to provide an integrated perspective on life which incorporated the material and spiritual aspects and practices.
Nevertheless what we find in actual practice is that there is a vast majority whose religion/philosophy/way of life is not the same as those of the few specialists. Iyengar (1909), in his Outlines of Indian Philosophy, referring to Hindu tradition, has made the following assertion, which elucidates this point well. “Though the Hindu venerates the Veda as self-revealed and takes pride in calling himself a Vedānti and has recently proposed to himself to carry the light of Vedānta to the West, yet the living religion of India, what one may call the ‘working faith’ of the Hindus of today, is based on the Āgamas, Śaiva, Śaktā, or Vaiśnava…” (Preface, p. iv). Āgamas are those which prescribe the rules and regulations related to rituals and worship. Even after a century since these observations were made, the Āgamas seem to have a greater influence on our thought patterns and life style, as compared to the Vedas. Pūja (ritual), bali (sacrifice), homa (a sacrificial offering of ghee) and yagna (ritual of sacrifice) have been an integral part of life of the Indian people even today. Rituals and offerings are the predominant ways of relating to the divinity by the masses, be it in Buddhism, Hinduism, or Jainism. Details may differ from one religion to another, but the essence is the same. Lord Mahavīra and the other Tīrthankaras and the Buddha are also worshipped in temples, like the Hindu gods and goddesses. When we consider other practices we find the same tendency. For instance, the Kumbhamela has a greater mass appeal than the teachings of Sri Ramakrishna Paramahaṃsa or Sri Ramana Maharshi. Festivals such as Śivarātri and Krishna Janmāshṭamī draw more crowds to temples as compared to the study of Upaniṣads. Similarly many rituals connected with birth and death, initiation, marriage, and other ceremonies have elements of pre-Vedic and Vedic rituals. Even among those who seek to go beyond the ordinary life, to realize a higher reality, we find more people opting to practice asceticism, wandering mendicancy, yoga, etc., than following the philosophies of the Vedānta. Many of these practices are clearly influenced by the iconic religion of pre-Vedic-non-Aryan people. Thus, we have many sādhus, sanyāsis, yogis, but not as many Vedāntins in India. Vedānta is not the religion of common man. Either it is considered as philosophical and other worldly or is not suited for the layman. Whilst on one hand the Indian masses display a culture of faith, devotion, and worship, which seem to have been inherited from pre-Vedic period, on the other hand, inquiry concerning the nature of reality, self, life, as pursued by Vedic and post-Vedic philosophers has progressed in a different direction to be pursued by a few. This distinction is crucial in understanding the Indian psyche and also in developing Indian psychological thought.
Dandekar (1941/1981) observes that it is inadequate to seek the origins of all Indian thought in Vedas alone and notes that concepts such as atomism has no Vedic origin. Further, he observes that a critical study of Sāṁkhya , Yoga, Vaiśeṣika, Buddhism, and Jainism would show that “there existed in these systems a substratum of common concepts which were not definitely derived from the Veda but which might be traced back to a pre-Vedic Magadhan thought-complex” (p. 339). However, he has not elaborated upon what is its nature and hence it is difficult to state in what way Vedic and pre-Vedic thought differed. For our purpose it may be worthwhile to consider Vedic and these other traditions as two different streams of thought parallel to each other, representing some fundamental distinctions. Considering the underlying concepts and processes, it may also be necessary to distinguish the Vedas, Upaniṣads, Pūrva Mīmāṃsa and Vedānta from Sāṁkhya , Yoga, Vaiśeṣika, Buddhism and Jainism, although traditional scholars group them as orthodox and heterodox as already noted. In order to understand the underlying thought process we need to examine the origins of Veda that gives some definite clue.
Śruti and SmṚti
Though it is said that pre-Vedic culture had significant influence on ancient Indians, it is conventional to trace the first literary manifestations of Indian thought to the Vedas (Dandekar, 1959/1981). The Vedas are basically oral in nature and they are mantras produced spontaneously, not thought out. They are also called śruti which in Sanskrit means “that which is heard”. What is implied here is that the mantras, also called ṛk, are known to have always existed independently in the universe, and may be as patterns of sound vibrations and that the sages have just perceived them acoustically. That is, ṛṣis did not compose the Vedas, but they cognized them. Hence, they are known as mantra draśtā, ‘seers’ of the mantra. For example, the famous Gāyatri mantra which addresses Sāvitr (Sun God) was perceived by Sage Vishwāmitra, in a particular meter termed as Gāyatri chhandas (hence the mantra is named after that meter). This is the traditional understanding. Barua (1921) notes that Buddha regarded the following ten sages as the ancient and real authors and reciters of the mantras: Astaka, Vāmaka, Vāmadeva, Vishwāmitra, Yamadagni, Āngirasa, Bharadvāja, Vasiśta, Kaṣyapa and Bhṛgu. It was their duty to invoke several deities, such as Indra, Soma, Varuna, Īsāna, Prajāpati, Brahma, Mahiddhi and Yama. The relation between ṛṣi, mantra, and devatā is of an integral nature.
Veda is derived from the Sanskrit root vid, to know, and hence also represents knowledge, whose origin is śruti (cosmic sounds of truth). Thus, in Indian epistemology śabda prāmānya or śruti prāmānya (“scripture” as source of valid knowledge) refers to Veda as authority. Hence ṛṣi vākya, utterance of the seer, carried the stamp of authority and was relied upon. From a psychological point of view it may be worthwhile to raise a few questions on the nature of the Vedas. What is that experience of hearing? What are the contents of what one hears? Can one have them now? Does it represent any specific mode of cognition or mentality? How does it attain the status of valid knowledge?
Julian Jaynes (1976), in his theory on the origins of human consciousness, has noted that Veda is an example of a particular type of human mentality that existed before the 2nd millennium B.C.E., which he calls bicameral mind. Based on extensive archeological, ethnographic, anthropological, historical, linguistic and biological evidences collected from ancient civilizations across the globe, Jaynes postulates that in the bicameral era ancient peoples from Mesopotamia to Peru could not “think” as we do today, and were therefore not conscious. Unable to introspect, they experienced auditory hallucinations, voices of gods actually heard as in the Old Testament or the Iliad, which, coming from the right hemisphere of the brain told a person what to do in circumstances of novelty or stress. This ancient mentality he has called the bicameral mind and notes that bicameral cultures and bicameral kingdoms existed up to second millennium B.C.E.
Summarizing his arguments Jaynes states: “I have endeavored…to examine the record of a huge time span to reveal the plausibility that man and his early civilizations had a profoundly different mentality from our own; that in fact men and women were not conscious as are we, were not responsible for their actions, and therefore cannot be given the credit or blame for anything that was done over these vast millennia of time; that instead each person had a part of his nervous system which was divine, by which he was ordered about like any slave, a voice or voices which indeed were what we call volition and empowered what they commanded and were related to the hallucinated voices of others in a carefully established hierarchy…The total pattern, I suggest, is in agreement with such a view. It is, of course, not conclusive6” (p. 201-202).
Further, he notes that in the bicameral era man had no internal ‘space’ in which to be private and no analog ‘I’ to be private with and all initiative were in the voices of gods. There were no private ambitions, grudges, and frustrations. The bicameral mind was the social control, not fear or repression or even law. Within each bicameral state, the people were probably more peaceful and friendly than in any civilization since that time. The problems were complex and quite different at the interfaces of different bicameral civilizations (Jaynes, 1976).
However, the second millennium B.C.E did not last that way. There were wars, catastrophes and national migrations. Hierarchies crumpled. “Between the act and its divine source came the shadow, the pause that profaned, the dreadful loosening that made the gods unhappy, recriminatory, jealous. Until finally, the screening off of their tyranny was effected by the invention on the basis of language of an analog space with an analog ‘I’. The careful elaborate structures of the bicameral mind had been shaken into consciousness” (p. 204). Then onwards the gods needed to be assisted by their divinely dictated laws only in the late federations of states.
Referring to the Vedas he states, “In India, the oldest literature is the Veda, which was dictated by gods to the ṛṣi or prophets; these too were poetry…Poetry then was divine knowledge… The poet and divine seer have a long tradition of association in the ancient world, and several Indo-European languages have a common term for them…” Jaynes further notes that “among the early Arabic peoples, the word for poet was sha’ir, ‘the knower’, or a person endowed with knowledge by the spirits; his metered speech in recitation was the mark of its divine origin….Poetry then was the language of gods” (p. 361-364). No wonder that we have many terminologies associated with the Vedas like, Śruti, Mantradraśta, Kavi, Udghośa, Udgeeta, Devatā, Chandas, Śrutipramāna, and others, which refer to aspects of bicameral mind that Jaynes has postulated. After the breakdown of the bicameral mind, poetry was the sound and tenor of authorization. Yet, it is doubtful whether the Vedas really are products of a bicameral mind and whether they were dictated by Gods because he has made another observation about the Vedas and Upaniṣad: “Indian hurtles from the bicameral Veda into the ultra subjective Upaniṣads , neither of which are authentic to their times” (Jaynes, 1976, p. 313). This observation is very important because Jaynes recognizes that Veda is not necessarily hundred percent bicameral. It also shows that the Vedas could have been of a later date than what Jaynes terms as bicameral era, though it has bicameral features.
The idea that the Vedas are a product of a different type of human cognitive ability finds support in Sri Aurobindo. Sri Aurobindo (1956) reported that he had a unique auditory experience when he was in jail as a freedom fighter, for which he had not found sufficient explanation either in European psychology or in the teachings of Yoga or Vedānta. Only later in Pondicherry when he read the Vedas for the first time Veda mantras illuminated with a clear and exact light the psychological experiences he had in the jail, and he realized his experience was an instance of cognizing of mantras.
Veda…is the creation of an age anterior to our intellectual philosophies. In that original epoch thought proceeded by other methods than those of our logical reasoning and speech accepted modes of expression which in our modern habits would be inadmissible. The wisest then depended on inner experience and the suggestions of the intuitive mind for all knowledge that ranged beyond mankind’s ordinary perceptions and daily activities. Their aim was illumination, not logical conviction, their ideal the inspired seer, not the accurate reasoner. Indian tradition has faithfully preserved this account of the origins of the Vedas.
Sri Aurobindo (1956, p.11).
Further, Sri Aurobindo opines that Vedas are the ancient psychological science and the art of spiritual living and the Upaniṣads are the philosophical outcome and modification of Vedas and Vedānta, Sāṁkhya and Yoga are late intellectual result and logical dogma. Sri Aurobindo’s experiences and observations attests to the traditional claim that Veda mantras exist in their own right in the universe and they are accessible any time under certain conditions. It supports the belief in Indian tradition that Vedas are apauruśeya, i.e., of non-human origin. The Vedas seem to have some unique knowledge, which neither modern psychology nor Yoga and Vedānta can help us to understand and hence we surmise that their cognitive bases are different. The Vedas stand in their own right.
A question that arises is whether bicameral mind and intuitive mind are identical. Jaynes proposes that there are three kinds of human awareness: (a) the bicameral or god-run man, (b) the modern or problem-solving man, and (c) contemporary forms of throwbacks to bicamerality, such as hypnotism, schizophrenia, poetic and religious frenzy and other such phenomena. But he is not clear about the intuitive mind. Then what exactly is the mental faculty from which the Vedas have emerged?
Sharma (1973) in his discussions on the age of the Ṛg Veda notes that there are four different lines of approach for determining it: linguistic data, geographical conditions, archaeological evidence and astronomical evidence. Referring to the linguistic data he notes that according to Yāska, the author of Nirukta, the Vedas were revealed to ancient seers and sages and were taught to the first three generations through the oral tradition. Later generations declined the oral instruction for want of intuition and required texts which could explain to them the Vedic meaning and other things connected with it. As a result, six auxiliary treatises, the Vedāngas, were developed.
Sri Aurobindo (1956) notes that there was a change in the mental mode with the beginning of the Upaniṣadic period, which is dated as 1000 B.C when the “Age of Intuition” was passing away into the first dawn of the “Age of Reason”. The Upaniṣadic ṛṣis sought to recover the lost or waning knowledge through meditation and spiritual experience. They used the text of the ancient mantras as a prop or an authority for their own intuitions and perceptions. The Vedic word was a seed of thought and vision by which they recovered old truths in new forms. We can also recount here that Lannoy (1971) termed Indian thinking as hylozoistic which involves some kind of immediate awareness, which Sri Aurobindo terms as “knowledge by identity”. Prasad (1958, p. 82) notes that the dialogue and discourses that characterized the Upaniṣadic period naturally resulted in the formulation of definite methods of debating and forms of reasoning, which gave rise to a science originally called ānviksiki (the science of enquiry), then tarka-vidyā (science of reasoning), and ultimately Nyāya-Śāstra (the science of logic). Ānviksiki began as a science of general enquiry, which included in its scope both metaphysics and logic. Later on it assumed a more specified form and became the science of pure reasoning. The transitions from the predominance of intuition to the intellect mode seem to have reached stability during the period when the Nyāya school of thought came into predominance.
What came out of this gradual transition in the mode of thought and of subsequent period are collectively called smṛti. It means that which was recovered, recollected, recognized, and remembered. The insights recovered by Upaniṣadic seers and sages were further elaborated, interpreted, commented, and explained in different sources. They include dharma śāstra, nibandha, purāṇa, itihāsa, āgama and tantra, vedānga and upa veda, sat darsana, and other scriptures like Yoga Vāsishta. Sat darsana includes Mīmāṃsa Sūtra, Brahma Sūtra (Vedānta Sūtra), Sāṁkhya Sūtra, Yoga-Sūtra, Nyāya Sūtra, and Vaiśeṣika Sūtra. Vedānga includes śikṣā, vyākarana, kalpa śāstra, nirukta, chandas śāstra,and jyotiśa. Upa Veda includes āyur veda, dhanur vidyā, artha śāstra and gandharva veda (Pandit, 2001). Smṛtis are derived from śruti and hence they are secondary sources. The tradition of śruti and smṛti lead togeneration of innumerable treatises on all subjects: physics, mathematics, astronomy, chemistry, biology, political science, economics, art, literature, linguistics, medicine, philosophy, theology, and spirituality. In addition, scholars from other traditions like Jainism and Buddhism also contributed their insights on the above subjects. Alongside, there is the Siddha tradition, which is primarily of Dravidian origin, that address different subjects mentioned (Shaktidharan and Kumar, 2001). The popular belief that Siddha is a system of medicine is a narrow understanding of the tradition.
The distinction between śruti and smṛti is very important from the psychological and epistemological view point. The distinction between the revealed and the recovered has also lead to a distinction between two types of knowledge viz., pratyakṣa jñāna (direct and immediate) and parokṣa jñāna (indirect and mediated). Much of Indian theory of knowledge and perception centre on this fundamental distinction and has significant implications for cognitive psychology.
Content of the Vedas
Ṛg Veda, which is the first and ancient among the four Vedas is the store house of earliest poetry that arose among the Indo-Aryan and in this collection one finds the highest thoughts, priestly poetry, spells, philosophical thoughts and also purely secular compositions (Sharma, 1973). From the psychological point of view, Sri Aurobindo (1956) notes that the contents of Vedic hymns have lead to the formulation of the notions of states of consciousness and corresponding worlds (lokas). They are seven subjective principles of consciousness and corresponding seven worlds. The principles are sat, chit, ānanda, vijñāna, manas, prāṇa, and anna. The corresponding principles of existence are satyaloka, tapoloka, janoloka, maharloka, swaraloka, bhuvarloka,and bhūloka. In other words, the Vedas propounded what may be called a ‘theory of psycho-cosmic correspondence’. Sri Aurobindo states that human beings have subjective faculties hidden in them, which correspond to all the tiers and strata of the subjective cosmic system and they form for us so many planes of our possible existence. In his view this material life and our narrowly limited consciousness of the physical world are not the sole experience permitted to man. He can awaken to profounder depths and higher heights within and such awakening is his intended progress.
William James (1902/1968) had earlier come to a similar conclusion after examining the varieties of religious experiences. He stated that normal waking consciousness, rational consciousness as we call it, is but one special type of consciousness. Surrounding it but separated from it by the “filmiest of screens” there lie potential forms of consciousness that are entirely different and of which we are not aware. But if we apply the requisite stimulus they come to our experience as definite types of mentality, which probably somewhere have their field of application and adaptation. James says, no account of the universe in its totality can be final without recognizing them and they forbid a premature closing of our accounts with reality. The reality of the lokas or potential forms of consciousness/reality is disputed, however, some of the first-person accounts of altered states of consciousness in modern times and the theory following that seem to confirm their validity (Grof, 2006).
The inference we can draw from these two observations is that Vedic ṛṣi had explored all possible states and their understanding of psyche was vastly different in nature and scope from that of modern psychology. They had not closed their account with reality prematurely. This is reflected in the postulate of pure consciousness (turīyā) as distinct from the three states: jāgrat, swapna, and suśupti, discussed in Brhadāranyaka and Māndukya Upaniṣads .
Vedic, Post-Vedic, Neo-Vedic-And-Sophistic Philosophies And Philosophers
Traditional scholars of India mark four distinct periods in the transition from śruti to smṛti . They are Samhitā, Brāhmana, Āranyaka/Upaniṣad and Sūtra-Vedānga periods (Dandekar, 1959/1981). In tracing the history of Indian philosophy in pre-Buddhistic period, Barua (1921) divides the stages of development into Vedic, post-Vedic, and neo-Vedic-and-Sophistic periods. Each of them has one primary focus viz., cosmological, physico-psychological, and logico-ethical, in that order. In between two main periods there were transition periods. Barua makes a distinction between ‘early’ Vedic ṛṣis, already cited, and ‘later’ Vedic ṛṣis, whom he calls as philosophers. In his scheme of classification, Vedic philosophy is constituted primarily of Samhitā and some Brāhmana texts. The important ṛṣis who are considered as Vedic philosophers by him are Aghamarśana, Prajāpati Paramesthin, Brahmanaspati, Anila, Dīrghatamas, Nārāyana, Hiranyāgarbha and Viṣvakarman. The chief interest of the Vedic thinkers was centered upon the physical world as a whole.
The history of the post-Vedic period includes the works of a few ancient Brāhmana schools such as the Aitareyas, the Cāndogyas, the Kausitakeyas, the Taittirīyas, and the Satapathas. The important ṛṣis considered as post-Vedic philosophers are Mahidāsa Aitareya, Suravīra-Sākalya, Māndukeya-Kaunthāravya, Raikva, Bādhva, Śāndilya, Satyakāma, Jābala, Jaivali, Gārgāyana, Prātardhāna, Uddālaka, Varuna, Bālāki, Ajātasatru, and Yāgnavalkya. According to Barua, the principal combatants in philosophy in this period were Brāhmins and Kṣatriyas. He places the lower limit of this period nearer to 900 B.C.E, considering the process of thought-evolution. The chief concerns of the post-Vedic thinkers are the organic world and man.
The neo-Vedic-and-Sophistic philosophical period, according to Barua, stretched from 900 B.C.E up to the time of Mahāvīra, the 24th Tīrthankara and the founder of Jainism as a formal religion. The neo-Vedic period is the Sūtra period, as per the history of Indian literature. In the history of Indian religions it is the period of Śramanas and Brāhmins or wanderers and hermits. This period “was so far removed from the ancient Vedic tradition that thinkers had in course of time ceased to feel the fascination of, and cherish admiration for, Vedic learning and Vedic rites” (ibid. p. 193). These were the Metaphysicians (Āsuri, Pippalāda, Bharadvāja, Naciketas, Pūrna Kaśyapa, Kakuda Kātyāyana, Ajita Keśakambalin, and Maskarin Gośāla); the Skeptics (Sanjaya) and the Moralists (teachers of ethical morals--Vātsayāna; teachers of political morals--Kautilya; and the teachers of juristic morals).
Systems and Schools of Indian Psychology
It is premature to speak of systems and schools of Indian psychology. However, it is possible to identify certain trends in ancient thought that later developed into different philosophical systems from which we can derive schools and systems of psychology. For this the stages of philosophical development are important. It is evident that much of the Upaniṣadic thinking that has formed the basis for both Sāṁkhya and Vedānta systems which are considered as primary sources of Indian psychological thought happened in the post-Vedic period. By neo-Vedic period the allegiance for the Vedas as authority had waned and philosophers of this time were independent thinkers who came up with their own reflections that often deviated and was in contradiction to Vedic lore and insights. It is therefore necessary to keep Vedic and post-Vedic thought development separate from neo-Vedic thought development.
It is necessary to distinguish Veda mantras and Vedic philosophy. Veda mantras can be adequately delved into to understand more about the transpersonal dimension of human existence and the dimension of psyche and consciousness as Sri Aurobindo’s observations suggest. Vedic psychology itself can exist in its own right because of its intuitive cognitive basis. As regards Vedic philosophy, which is less intuitive and more intellectual, each of the three periods identified by Barua has its own focus and physico-psychological problems were the focus of post-Vedic philosophers. He points out that though Vedic philosophers primarily focused on cosmological issues, Dīrgatamas’ hymns anticipated the problems of the post-Vedic period. Some of them include the expression “What thing I am I know not clearly”, conception of truth (satya) and right (dharma) as ṛta denoting the eternal order of things (X.85); the conception of faith (śraddha)as the yearning of the heart for better condition of existence (X. 85); the vague notion of the four stages of the development of the foetus in the womb (X.85); and the equally vague notion of rebirth and the two paths, devayāna and pitryāna, along which the soul after death proceeds to its destination” (Barua, 1921, p. 413).
Referring to the problems of post-Vedic philosophy Barua states that on the contemplative side “the main question with the early post-Vedic thinkers appertained to Yoga - the inner culture of faith and intellect” (p. 44). It is related to the religious aspiration of man to unite with what is divine in nature and it was never absent from the Vedic or Indo-Aryan minds. The fundamental question, “How can I unite with him?” might have lead to two distinct problems of philosophy. (1) Who is he with whom I shall unite? (2) Who am I who shall unite with him? Here was the beginning of Indian psychology of self and identity. Another characteristic of this period was the development of logic (tarka) and dialectic (mīmāṁsa) that has seeds of Indian psychology of cognition.
Barua draws our attention to a distinction between the chronology of literature and that of thought, which I think is useful from the view point of developing systems and schools of psychology. He notes that there are some schools such as the Aitareya Brāhmana and the Āryankas, omitting the Upaniṣad portion, which represent together a homogenous body of doctrines that may be judged as the formulation of a particular individual or of a particular school of thought, i.e., of Mahidāsa Aitareya or the Aitareya School. On the other hand, the Aitareya Upaniṣad contains the views of many other individuals and schools in addition to those of Aitareya School. Taittirīya Brāhmana, Āryanka and Upaniṣad represent the views of Taittirīyas only and hence can be termed as Taittirīya School. Varuna, father of Bhṛgu Vāruni, is considered as the best exponent of Taittirīya school. However, Kausitiki, Brhadāranyaka, and Candogya-Brāhmana Upaniṣads are compilations containing the views of several teachers, differing in content from one another (Barua, 1921). Such distinctions indicate that it is possible for us to trace and understand the different psychological views available in Indian tradition to Aitareya, Taittirīya and other schools. Aitareya and Taittirīya schools have their origin in Ṛg Veda and Krishna Yajurveda respectively. Aitareya school is earlier to Taittirīya school.
Considering these differences we have to treat Vedic and post-Vedic philosophy as one unit contributing to a major movement of psychological thought and neo-Vedic as another contributing to a different line of thinking. It is likely that neo-Vedic thought was influenced more by pre-Vedic traditions. Hence, Vedic and post-Vedic thought and neo-Vedic thought could be treated as precursors for two independent systems of psychology, within which we can identify many schools. These different systems have affirmed monistic or dualistic world-view and it is not necessarily characteristic of either of the systems. For instance, we have Advaita Vedānta (Non-dualism) and Dvaita Vedānta (Dualism) both of which belong to the Vedic stream. Neo-Vedic philosophers seem to have influenced Jaina and Bouddha traditions more. If we accept the suggestion of Ajaya (1983) that Indian systems can be hierarchically arranged beyond Piaget’s formal operational thought as “dualistic” and “unitive”, then all Indian thought systems may be considered to have emerged from post Piagetian stages, be it different schools of Vedānta, Sāṁkhya, Yoga, and others except that of Cārvaka school, which is out and out materialistic and reductionist. Hence, the primary and fundamental assumptions and tenets of systems and schools of Indian thought cannot be understood with reference to modern scientific psychology and its systems.
Just as we have specific schools and theories focused on one or the other aspects, like behaviorism was focused on learning, gestalt theory on perception, humanist psychology on personality and growth, psychoanalysis on unconscious, existentialism on the issue of meaning of life, free will and determinism and so on, the different systems and schools of Indian thought have also focused on certain processes and issues. Some of them are listed as examples as potential sources for developing specific theories and models of psychology. For instance, contemporarily both Western and Indian psychologists have concentrated on Vedānta and Yoga as psychologies of consciousness and transcendence. Upaniṣads and Vedānta can contribute more for the understanding of consciousness and self, from ordinary to extraordinary states and empirical to transcendental self. Similarly many Indian psychologists have already explored the concept and theory of triguṇa in Sāṁkhya for their potential to develop psychological typologies and have developed instruments (see Murthy and Kumar, 2007 for a review of this work). Along with the concepts ahaṁkāra, buddhi and citta, a comprehensive theory of motivation, personality and values can be developed. The many discussions on the theories of knowing and epistemology in Nyāya addresses cognitive processes like perception, reasoning and criteria of truth and right knowledge and also errors and distortions in cognition and hence can contribute to developing a cognitive psychology that also takes into account intuition as a valid source of knowledge. The Vaiśeṣika with its emphasis on determining the specific nature and property of objects and also uniqueness can help in developing a psychology of individual differences. Pūrva Mīmāṃsa with its emphasis on what is known as karma kānda and Bhagavad Gītā’s emphasis on niśkāma karma can help develop theories of human motivation and action, of control and of personal efficacy. Dharma Śāstra and Gṛhya Sūtras speak about one’s social life, conduct, and ethics and also about social institutions like marriage and family, which can help develop developmental and social psychology. Nātyaśāstra of Bharata and the entire Sanskrit literary works can help us develop an understanding of human affect. Vyākaraṇa is psycholinguistics. Rāmāyaṇa, Yogavasiśṭha, Mahābhārata, Bhagavad Gītā can provide insights into the practical aspects of dealing with life’s problems and help develop counseling psychology. The Āyurveda samhitās can be used to develop an understanding of biological processes, mental illness and therapy. This list can be continued but is not meant to be exhaustive and readers may think further on these lines.
Notions about psyche, mind and mental functions
So far, this chapter has discussed the historical influences in the development of Indian thought, their characteristics and the potential inherent in them to develop systems and schools of Indian psychology. Now it may be worthwhile to pay attention to the concept of mind itself. As mentioned, the Indian sages and seers were weary of pursuing the inquiry into the nature of mind for its own sake and did so in the context of realizing the ultimate reality termed as Brahman in the Vedic tradition. Although Jainism and Buddhism have dwelt on this subject in great detail, the focus in this section is on the historical development of these concepts.
It was noted in the section on the characteristics of Indian psyche that primitive societies were characterized by hylozoistic thinking, as Lannoy puts it, which refers to a tendency to draw no clear distinction between matter, life and mind and represented a sense of unity of all life. For instance, with reference to the language of the Iliad, Jaynes (1976, p. 69) notes: “The word psyche, which later means soul or conscious mind, is in most instances life-substances, such as blood or breath: a dying warrior bleeds out his psyche into the ground or breathes it out in his last gasp. The thumos, which later comes to mean something like emotional soul, is simply motion or agitation. When a man stops moving, the thumos leaves his limbs. But it is also somehow like an organ also…”Similarly, words such as phren, usually used in the plural as phrenes, is anatomically localized as the midriff, or sensations in the midriff. It is another term that has a similar use, which centuries later comes to mean mind or ‘heart’ in its figurative sense. Noos, which is spelled as nous in later Greek, is derived from the word nooein, to see, and it later comes to mean conscious mind.
In another context Jaynes (1976, p. 271) observes that the term psyche has probably come from pschein, to breathe, and it has become internalized into life substances in its main usage in the Iliad and has been used in the sense of life. He hastens to add that the usage is not as time span between birth and death. Instead, the emphasis is on vitality or that which makes living possible . He cites the following examples from the Iliad (6th - 7th century BC): “When a spear strikes the heart of a warrior his psyche dissolves (5:296), is destroyed (22:325), or simply leaves him (16; 453), or is coughed out through the mouth (9:409), or bled out through a wound (14: 548, 16:505)….it (psyche) is very simply a property that can be taken away, and is similar to the taking away, under the same conditions, of thumos or activity, a word with which psyche is often coupled.” That is, life and activity go together. “In a sense, psyche is the most primitive of these preconscious hypostases; it is simply the property of breathing or bleeding or what not in that physical object over there called a man or animal, a property which can be taken from him like a prize (22:161) by a spear in the right place.” Thus, the linguistic origins of mentalist notions clearly betray an organic/biological basis to them, in the Western tradition.
The connotation of the term psyche has developed in five stages in Greek history from the period of Homer (6th-7th century BCE) to 5th century CE. During this long interval of almost 3500 years the term has been used to refer to: (a) a kind of breath which is blown out at death; (b) the seat of emotion; (c) intellectual ‘interpreter’ of sense data; (d) moral as well as intellectual faculty; and (e) to ‘a person and within a person a soul’. The term has also been used to denote a thing, a process or an agent (personal or divine). While Plato (428/427 BCE–348/347 BCE) understood psyche or soul as an immaterial entity which apprehends an ideal world, Aristotle (384 BC–322 BCE) used soul and mind synonymously and regarded psyche as process, form, or function and defined it in terms of its activity (Cohen, 1972). The contemporary usage is clearly Aristotelian. There seem to be a lot of parallels in the way notions about psyche and mind have developed in the west and in India. In Indian tradition too, Vedic literature provides us many notions related to psyche and mind, some of which are strikingly parallel to the Greek notions, both in terms of their origin and meaning and very much betray an organic/biological basis to them.
Asu, Prāna and Manas
Dandekar (1941/1981) states that primitives believed in a universal all-penetrating magic fluid like aronda or the Vedic asu, the amount of which, and not the possession or non-possession of any special mental or physical faculties, determined the gradation among beings in the universe. S. K. R. Rao (1962, p.5) notes that asu is an energizing influence, it is a distinct principle, distinct from the body and devoid of it the body is dead8 and men are described as “possessors of asu”. Hence, asu is synonymous with psyche as used in ancient times. Another term that has direct parallel with psyche is prāna. Prāna is a physiological reality and is the essential vital constituent in the organism.
Yet another term for the magic fluid is mana which was supposed to have the capacity to bestow upon beings some kind of occult power which made them ‘men’. The words manas, manus, man, mensch, etc., have come into existence as the result of the influence of primitive thought and those words did not originally denote exclusively psychological or non-material notions. In many of the Ṛg vedicpassages, manas suggests a dualism with the physical body, not in the sense of ‘cogitatio’ and ‘extensio’, but in the sense of a potence-bestowing substance and its substratum. The loss of manas results in virtual death. In early Vedic literature manas is often represented as being capable of modifications, which are usually associated with matter. For instance, manas is considered to be capable of movement in space and no function of manas is possible without some movement on its part. There are references to the ‘placing’ of manas; ‘yoking’ or ‘directing’ of manas; and ‘grasping’ of and by manas. Many more such usages are found. “Such descriptions may not be explained away as mere imaginative or poetical representations9of the activities of manas. They clearly betray the original ‘somatic’ nature of manas. Manas must have been regarded as a kind of ‘material substance’, and its activities were, therefore, necessarily described as mechanical and dynamic modifications of that ‘substance’” (Dandekar, 1941/1981, p. 246-50).
Regarding the organic or somatic basis of mental functions, Dandekar (1941/1981) noted that Indo-European people had fair knowledge of the human anatomy which has given rise to a large number of words in many Indo-European languages denoting several parts of the human body, which in turn is the basis of words denoting ‘sensations’, ‘emotions’, and other such non-physical (or psychological) phenomena. He cites the following examples. In Homeric language zred, zreues, prapides (connected with diaphragm) denote almost all the varieties of will, thought, understanding, etc. He suggests that the words kholos (anger), kerdos (advice, gain), and kerdosune (cunning, will) are connected with kardia (heart). Similarly, in later Greek, the entrails (dplankuizomai) seem to be regarded as the seat of feelings and emotions. The Latin words, vecors (wrong-sensed), recordari (remember), etc., indicate that those faculties were again connected with cor, cordis (heart). In the Slovanic languages, particularly Russian, anger and excitement are associated with liver. Even in Indian languages we can find many emotions and affective states being referred in terms of visceral organs.
The vedic word hṛd (heart) is frequently associated with manas and there are numerous references to mind being ‘located’ in the heart in Vedic texts. According to Dandekar (1941/1981) it is merely a rhyme–word for Indo-European ker (e) d, which has its linguistic counterpart in Gk. kardia,Lat. cor,Air. cride Got. hairto,etc. Linguistically the word hṛd has to be traced back to Indo-European ghrd which later has been preserved only in Aryan languages. In certain Vedic passages it also means ‘belly’ or ‘stomach’ Thus, he asserts that the “association of manas with human psychology is, therefore, clearly an afterthought” (p.247). S. K. R. Rao (1962) notes that when manas is used in association with hṛd it represents the stirred-up state of the individual and it is identified as the source of all involuntary, irrational and normally uncontrolled psychological processes such as desires and urges, emotions and moods -- in general, the affective aspect of human nature
But, it is doubtful whether the Vedic hṛd is the same as the physical organ located on the left side of the chest, because hṛd in some ancient spiritual literature refers to a location on the right side of the chest and is considered as the seat of the transcendental self. In modern times Sri Ramana Maharshi categorically states this and hastens to add that reference to a physical location is only an aid to understanding and ātman as transcendental Self has no meaning if it can be located in a physical space (1996, p.11 and 62).
According to Dandekar (1941/1981), in Vedic literature itself, the term manas is used in two distinct ways. He notes that in the Ṛg Veda, particularly in 57 and 58 sūkta of Tenth mandala, the term is used in the sense of a material substance and in Vajasaneyi-Samhitā 34 the term is used in the sense of the faculties of the human mind. Dandekar observes that the substance notion of manas persisted even in the Upaniṣads, Buddhism and Yoga, and influenced the way the nature and activities of psychological functions are represented. As he notes, Cāndogya Upaniṣad, (VI, 5.4:6.1-2) states “annamayam hi somya manaḥ” i.e., “manas is said to have originated out of the food that we eat” (p. 251). Similarly, in Sāṁkhya , the concept of prakṛti represents the fundamental material principle and 24 tattvas are supposed to have originated from this. Both mind and matter as differentiated in Western thought are considered here as two manifestations of prakṛti, constituted of triguṇa. It is necessary to recognize that the notion of materiality as understood in Indian tradition is not at the level of gross matter; what is referred to as a fundamental material principle is perhaps even subtler than what modern physicists call quark, charm, spin, which are subtler than fundamental particles. In Indian thought there are concepts like pancabhūta (five elements) and pancatanmātra (five subtle elements) that represent the subtlest of the subtle nature of matter.
Dandekar (1941/1981 p. 251) draws our attention to the three types of disturbances in psychical matter, from what he terms as the ‘somatic’ standpoint: first is ‘movement’, ‘vibration’ or ‘oscillation’; the second is ‘solidification’ or ‘thickening’; and the third is that of ‘defilement’ or ‘affliction’. The first is indicated in citta vṛtti and as Dandekar points out, this is indicated in the many technical terms used in yoga. In yoga, all consciousness phenomena such as feelings, emotions, perception, conception, knowledge, are regarded merely as either disturbances in or modifications of the psychical substance. Citta, referred to by Patañjali as mind, instead of manas,is pariṇāmi,i.e., it is constantly suffering a thousand modifications and the terms vṛtti and pravṛtti are clearly indicative of this feature. Vṛtti means the activity of an object; pravṛtti means the going forward of the citta towards its object. Dhāraṇā is the literal binding of citta to the desired object; dhyāna is explained as the homogeneity of the stream of consciousness. According to Dandekar, the stream of consciousness is not a metaphorical imagery but it is a real flow of the waves of citta.10
The second, according to Dandekar, is represented in the gradation of the four psychological skandhas, viz., vijñāna, saṃskāra, vedanā,and samjñā. As Rao (1962) notes in Maitri Upaniṣad, the term skandha is used in the sense of “branches of a tree” (7, 11). The allusion to solidification or thickening is obvious in this usage. However, the Buddha employed the term khanda instead of skandha, to designate the items in man’s personality which included both bodily and mental aspects. The psychological skandhas represent object experience (vedanā), perception (samjñā), volitional organization (samskāra), and consciousness (vijñāna). Here again we can see that there is a progressive “solidification” from object experience to consciousness in a metaphorical sense.
The third type of disturbance, defilement or affliction, is indicated by the notions of kleśa, āsrava, and prasāda. “According to the bhāsya on Yoga-Sūtra II, 11, citta, when defiled and afflicted through kleśa, is to be treated in the same manner as a dirty cloth. The Yogācāra school of Buddhism, represented by Maitreya Asanga, believes in the divisibility of citta. All these conceptions in later Indian psychology clearly remind one of the descriptions of manas and its activities in the early Vedic literature” (p. 251).
We should note here that though the Upaniṣads and Yoga are considered orthodox (i.e. having allegiance to the Vedas) and Buddhism as heterodox (i.e. not accepting the Veda as authority), they share some fundamental underlying notions that are more important in developing an Indian psychology than the conventional classification of darśana (view, system, or school). According to Dandekar, the substance notion of manas leaving its substratum, the physical body, and going forth to the object of comprehension has developed into an important feature of Indian epistemology and Indian psychology.
The implications of Dandekar’s thesis are as follows. First, the primitives and our early Vedic ancestors probably experienced reality without a notion of separate mental faculty. In other words, they were not ‘thinking beings’ in the modern sense of the term. Second, neither can we derive a psychology with a ‘mentalist’ notion from the early Vedic literature, nor can we superimpose one such psychology on our ancestors. Both are misguided attempts. Third, we have to labor hard to find out how and when the transition took place from materialist to mentalist usage of the term manas, which is not just a linguistic phenomenon, but probably a fundamental change in the nature of experience of our ancestors. This last one is an interesting problem of Vedic psychology, according to Dandekar, which he has not attempted to answer.
Here an analogy from the field of computer science may be useful. We know today how the computer has changed in its structure and function from just an aid for mathematical calculation, inspired by the abacus used in ancient times, to a binary digit electronic processing machine that can be used to create art and music, literary works, scientific experiments and even perform virtual worship using designated software. Thus, the contemporary concept of a computer certainly does not match with what it was when it originated almost a century ago. In a similar way even the notions of manas has passed through many vicissitudes like computers, both in the West and in India.
Ātman and Jīva
Another important term in Indian tradition that has similar connotations with psyche is ātman. Although not frequently mentioned in the Ṛg Veda (1400 BCE to 900 BCE), there are many sūktas devoted to it in the Atharva Veda (about 900 BCE). As S. K. R. Rao (1962) notes, the origin of the word ātman remains obscure, although there is a Greek word structurally similar to ātman,‘atmos’, meaning ‘smoke’, ‘vapor’. He suggests that Vedic ātman may be an alternate expression for prāṇa or asu, both of which indicate life, the former signifying the actual vital process and the latter abstract vital principle. Rao observes that the term ātman lends itself to different interpretations depending upon where the emphasis is laid. For instance, Vedic commentator Sāyana derives the word from the root an, which signifies the breathing process and according to Vopadeva it signifies movement or action. This emphasis leads to the interpretation that ātman is the dynamic principle of breathing. On the other hand, Nirukta (the branch of knowledge related to semantics of Sanskrit words) attaches importance to the root at, which means ‘to spread’, ‘to pervade’, ‘to fill’. Rao further notes that modern scholars “are attracted by the suggestive ending of the word with ‘+man’, which renders it a nomina actionis: the importance of the expression consists in its indication of the spirit’s capacity for action” (1962, p. 6).
The emphases laid on breathing, movement, action, pervasiveness, filling, and enlivening, lead to the understanding of ātman as the most essential or central vital principle and it parallels the ancient concept of psyche. This ancient distinction between body and a principle separate from it, which is responsible for life and activity, is found commonly all over the world. While modern psychology has banished this separate principle, it has been elevated as the highest principle in Indian tradition in the Upaniṣads. Ānandagiri, a later philosopher, has provided a synthetic view of the centrality of ātman that was current in a rudimentary form in Vedic age. Rao has summarized it as follows: “Ātman fills (the body), receives (the impressions from the outer world), and enjoys (or experiences) the presentational objects” (1962, p. 6). This marks the crucial distinction between modern psychology and Indian psychology.
Another ancient term that has parallels with psyche is jīva. Jīva represents life as against nirjīva (lifeless). This is equivalent to animate and inanimate and it has become associated with the principle of consciousness/awareness in a living organism as against dead. It also has the connotations of energy, movement and action all derived from vitality or life principle. Though both ātman and jīva are associated with the life principle, the latter is conceived as the life force in an individual, which later comes to mean the empirical self. Ātman, on the other hand, is understood as the fundamental principle of the universe in the Upaniṣads, which represents both awareness and energy. Even mental functions are all considered modifications of ātman only (Aitareya Upaniṣad, Chapter III, śloka 1-4)11. It is important to realize that a single principle is upheld as governing everything and ātman, jīva, and all mental activities are viewed as manifestations of Brahman (supreme spirit/consciousness) alone in different functional ways.
The concept of ātman is questioned in other Indian systems, which has significant implications for the development of psychological thinking. Even within the Vedic tradition from which Upaniṣads and schools of Vedānta have emerged, we find only the Advaita Vedānta School not accepting the ‘reality’ of the distinction between ātman and jīva in ontological sense, but other schools admitting them.
In summing up this section it can be said that Indian tradition has concepts parallel to the Greek psyche and made similar distinctions in understanding behavior. The distinction made between deha (gross physical body) on the one hand and manas and ātman on the other in early Ṛg Vedic period was the first precursor of later psychological thinking. This tripartite distinction can be found as body, mind and soul in English language also. Unique to Indian tradition and ethos is the concept of ātman. Its implication is so vast that it remains an integral part of Indian thinking--classical and folk--even today, ātman, manas, and deha are in the linguistic repertoire of all Indians--illiterate and literate. While Western thinking progressively veered towards body centeredness, Indian tradition remained predominantly spirit centered, and that is the foundation of Indian psychological thought.
Models in Indian Thought
William James (1950), in his definition of psychology states that it is a science of mental life, both of its phenomena and of their conditions. Feelings, desires, cognitions, etc., are the phenomena. Their nature, how and when they occur and their interrelationships are the conditions. In their attempts to answer these questions enlightened persons through the ages and different cultures have proposed different views. Marx and Hillix (1978) have preferred to call such views ‘models’ rather than ‘theories’, since the latter term is often used in the context of empirical observation through senses and mental activity is not accessible for such observations. There are many models of mind both in the West and in the East. Model, in an ordinary sense, also implies concrete sensory representations through imagery, diagrams, drawings, analogy and metaphor and also three dimensional visualizations or abstract conceptualizations. In recent times authors have used phrases like “Maps of the Mind” (Turner,1981) and “Metaphors of Consciousness” (Valle and Ekartsberg, 1989) to represent the different viewpoints in modern psychology. In the Indian tradition, pancakośa and kundalini cakras are examples of such models related to dimensions of personality and levels of consciousness states. Another model is about the relation of ātman, buddhi, manas and the five sense organs represented visually in the famous Gītopadeśa pictures, where Arjuna is in the chariot; Sri Krishna is in the seat of a charioteer holding the reins of five horses. We can find many more of them related to other aspects.
Any theory or model is based on certain fundamental assumptions regarding the nature of the subject about which it is developed. Models of mind are no exceptions and the differences that exist between Western and Eastern models are primarily due to such axiomatic differences. As understood by scholars and philosophers, models of mind available in India as contrasted with those available in the West differ significantly because of the experiential dimension from which the nature of mind is understood. Indian philosophical systems with the exception of the Cārvāka School, hold that human beings are capable of experiencing a dimension of awareness, which is beyond all mental processes, and affirm that as ‘pure consciousness’, with orthodox systems equating it with self-realization. A clear distinction is made between ‘pure consciousness’ and ‘mental processes’. The equation of ‘transcendent awareness’/‘pure consciousness’ with ‘true identity’ freed the ancient Indian ṛṣis from identifying themselves with this or that mental processes and thus allowed them to explore freely all the different mental processes. In this sense mental processes were ‘empirically observed as object’ by a ‘transcended subject’. Models proposed were thus more comprehensive and holistic (Kumar, 1989). Hence, a very important feature of Indian Psychology is the distinction made between śuddha caitanya or caitanya (consciousness) and manas (mind). Ancient thinkers accorded prominence to the former than to the latter and this difference has played a key role in the development of models in Indian psychological thought.
Conclusion
Tracing the development of psychological thought in India does not amount to identifying correspondences or isomorphic ideas to those of contemporary psychology. Instead, it would be an endeavor to bring to bold relief ideas and concepts, which may or may not correspond with that of modern psychology yet having its validity in human experience and contemporary context as well. Therefore, in developing Indian psychology, there is a need to distinguish between what is “common to all the schools” and what are the “unique aspects of different schools”, which will enable us to “delineate general characteristics” and identify the “central concerns”. It is a very arduous task and is perhaps the greatest challenge that we have to face in developing Indian psychology. Even more difficult is the task of tracing the exact origins and history of the development of psychological thought in Indian tradition, given its antiquity and lack of well established time periods for the sequence of events. The task at hand is formidable considering all the above limitations, yet worth the trouble.
References
Adair, J.G., Pandey, J., Begum, H.A., Puhan, B.N., & Vohra, N. (1995). Indigenization and development of the discipline: Perceptions and opinions of Indian and Bangladeshi Psychologists. Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology, 26(4), 392-407.
Ajaya, S. (1983). Psychotherapy east and west: A unifying paradigm. Honesdale, Pennsylvania: The Himalayan International Institute of Yoga Science and Philosophy of the U.S.A.
Akhilananda, S. (1948). Hindu psychology: It’s meaning for the west. London: George Routledge & Sons.
Akhilananda, S. (1952). Mental health & Hindu psychology. London: George Routledge & Sons.
Atreya, J. P. (1985). Mind and its functions in Indian thought. New Delhi: Classic Publishing Company.
Barua, B. M. (1921). A history of pre-Buddhistic Indian philosophy. Delhi: Motilal Banarsidass.
Bem, S., & Looren de Jong, H. (1997). Theoretical issues in psychology - An introduction. London: Thousand Oaks, New Delhi: Sage Publications.
Berry, J. W., Poortinga, Y. H., Segall, M., & Dasen, P. R. (1992). Cross-cultural psychology: Research and applications. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Capaldi, E. J., & Proctor, R. W. (1999). Contextualism in psychological research – A critical review. London: Thousand Oaks, New Delhi: Sage Publications.
Chakraborthy, S. K. (1991). Management by values: Towards cultural consequence. Delhi: Oxford University Press.
Chatterji, S. K., Pusalker, A. D., & Dutt, N. (1958). Editors’ preface. In The cultural heritage of India – Volume 1. Calcutta: The Ramakrishna Mission Institute of Culture.
Chennakeshavan, S. (1980). Concept of Mind in Indian Philosophy. Delhi: Motilal Banarasidass.
Cohen, J. (1972). The concept of mind. In T. E. Shanmugam (Ed.). The concept of mind. Madras: The Madras Psychology Society Publication.
Coster, G. (1934). Yoga and western psychology - A comparison. London: Oxford University Press (1998). Delhi: Motilal Banarsidass Publishers Private Limited.
Dalal, A.K. (2002). Psychology in India: A historical introduction. In G. Misra and A.K. Mohanty (Eds.), Perspectives on indigenous psychology. New Delhi: Concept Publication Company.
Dalal, A.S. (2001). A greater psychology: An introduction to the psychological thought of Sri Aurobindo. Pondicherry: Sri Aurobindo Ashram.
Dandekar, R. N. (1941/1981). Somatism of Vedic psychology. In Exercises in indology. Delhi: Ajanta Publishers.
Dandekar, R. N. (1959/1981). Indian pattern of life and thought - A glimpse of its early phases. In Exercises in indology. Delhi: Ajanta Publishers.
Dasgupta, S. N. (1927). Hindu mysticism. Delhi: Motilal Banrasidass.
David, Rhys, C. A. F. (1914). Buddhist psychology. London: G. Bell & Sons.
David, Rhys, C. A. F. (1936). The birth of Indian psychology and its development in Buddhism. London: G. Bell & Sons.
Gergen K.J., Gulerce A., Lock A., & Misra, G. (1996). Psychological science in cultural context. American Psychologist, 51(5), 496-503.
Groff, S. (2006). The ultimate journey: Consciousness and the mystery of death. Multidisciplinary Association for Psychedelic Studies (In Press).
Heimann, B. (1964). The facets of Indian thought. London: George Allen & Unwin.
Hong, Y., Morris, M. W., Chiu, C., & Benet-Martinez, V. (2000). Multicultural minds - A dynamic constructivist approach to culture and cognition. American Psychologist, 55 (7), 706-720.
Iyengar, P.T.S. (1909). Outlines of Indian philosophy. Vizagapatam (Vishakapatnam, India): Tara Press.
Jacobi, H. (1946). Studies in Jainism (Ed. Jinavijaya Muni). Ahmedabad: Jaina Sahitya Samsodhaka Karyalaya.
James, W. (1902/1968). The varieties of religious experience. New York: Fontana Library.
James, W. (1890/1950). The principles of psychology. New York: Dover Publications.
Jaynes, J. (1976). Origins of consciousness in the breakdown of the bicameral mind. New Jersey: Princeton University Press.
Kakar, S. (1996). The Indian psyche. New Delhi: Oxford University Press.
Kalghatgi, T.G. (1961). Some problems in Jaina psychology. Dharwar: Karnataka University.
Kim, U., & Park, Y.S. (2005). Integrated analysis of indigenous psychologies: Comments and extensions of ideas presented by Shams, Jackson, Hewang & Kashima. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 8, 75-95.
Kim, U., & Berry, J.W. (Eds.). (1993). Indigenous psychologies: Research and experience in cultural context. New Delhi: Sage publications.
Krishnan, L. (1994). Wanted – A value based psychology. Trends in Social Science Research, 1(1), 35-41.
Krishnan, L. (2003). The cognitive content of psychology in India. Psychological Studies, 48(3), 17-22.
Kumar, S. K. K. (1989). Models of mind in Indian thought. Paper presented in the UGC Seminar on Science and Monism. Bangalore University.
Kumar, S. K. K. (2001). Developing Indian psychological concepts: Paradigmatic issues. Workshop on Indian psychological insights on human welfare. Department of Psychology, Karnatak University, Dharwad.
Kumar, S. K. K. (in press). Indian indigenous perspectives: Developments and future possibilities. In G. Misra (Ed.). Fifth ICSSR survey of research in psychology. (In press).
Kuppuswamy, B. (1985). Elements of ancient Indian psychology. New Delhi, India: Vikas Publishing House.
Lannoy, R. (1971). The speaking tree: A study of Indian society and culture. London: Oxford University Press.
Marx, M. H., & Hillix, W. A. (1978). Systems and theories of psychology (2nd Ed.). New Delhi: Tata McGraw Hill Publication Company Limited.
Maslow, A. H. (1971). Farther reaches of human nature. New York: Penguin Books
Mehta, M. L. (1955). Jaina psychology. Amritsar
Misra, G. (2000). Implications of culture for psychological knowledge. In J. W. Berry, R. C. Misra, & R. C. Tripathi (Eds.). Psychology in human and social development, pp. 31-67. New Delhi: Sage publications.
Misra, G. (2001). Culture and self: Implications for psychological inquiry. Journal of Indian Psychology, 19 (1&2), 1-20.
Misra, G., Jain, V., & Singh, T. (1995). On the cultural frame of social cognition. Indian Psychological Abstracts and Reviews, 2, 1, 3-36.
Misra, G., & Mohanty, A.K. (Eds.). (2002). Perspectives on indigenous psychology. New Delhi: Concept Publishing Company.
Much, N. (1995). Cultural Psychology. In J.A. Smith, R. Harre, and L. Van Langenhove (Eds.), (1995a). Rethinking in psychology. London: Thousand Oaks, New Delhi: Sage Publications.
Murthy, P. K., & Kumar, S. K. K. (2007). Concept of guna - A critical analysis and synthesis. Psychological Studies, 52. (In press).
Naranjo, C., & Ornstein, R. E. (1971). On the psychology of meditation. New York: Viking Press.
Neki, J.S. (2000). Some thoughts on culture and psychotherapy. Psychological Studies 45(3), 126-130.
Ornstein, R. E. (1972). The psychology of consciousness. New York: Penguin.
Ornstein, R. E. (1973). (Ed.). The nature of human consciousness. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman and Company.
Pandey, J. (2001). Psychology in India: Past, present and future. Paper presented at the 12th Annual Conference of National Academy of Psychology, Kollam, Kerala.
Pandit, B. (2001). The Hindu mind. Delhi: New Age Books.
Paranjpe, A.C. (1984). Theoretical psychology: The meeting of east and west. New York: Plenum Press.
Paranjpe, A.C. (1998). Self and identity in modern psychology and Indian thought. New York: Plenum Press.
Prasad, J. (1958). History of Indian epistemology (2nd Edn.). Delhi: Munshiram Manoharlal.
Ramanujan, A. K. (1990). Is there an Indian way of thinking? An informal essay. In M. Marriot (Ed.). India through Hindu categories. New Delhi: Sage Publications.
Rao, K. R. (2005). Psychology in the Indian tradition: A classical model with contemporary relevance. Psychological Studies 50 (1), 1-8.
Rao, S. K. R. (1962). Development of psychological thought in India. Mysore: Kavyalya Publishers.
Safaya, R. (1975). Indian psychology: A critical and historical analysis of the psychological speculations in Indian philosophical literature. Delhi: Munshiram Manoharlal Publishers Pvt. Ltd.
Schwitzer, A. (1935). Die welatansuung der indischen denker mystik and ethik. Indian thought and its development. Hodder and Straughton (1936) (English Translation – C.E.B. Russell). 1956 Reprint - London: Adam Charles & Black.
Sen, I. (1986). Integral psychology. Pondicherry: Sri Aurobindo International Centre for Education.
Shaktidharan, R., & Kumar, S. R. (2001). Glimpses into the sources of Dravidian psychology. Souvenir – National Seminar on Psychology in India: Past, present and future, pp. 27. Fatima Mata National College, Kollam.
Sharma, S. N. (1973). A history of Vedic literature. Varanasi: The Chowkamba Sanskrit Series Office.
Sinha, D., & Tripathi, R. C. (1994). Individualism in a collectivist culture – A case of coexistence of opposites. In U. Kim, H. C. Triandis, C. Kagitsibasi, S. C. Choi, and G. Yoon (Eds.), Individualism and collectivism, pp. 123-136. New Delhi: Sage.
Sinha, D. (1993). Indigenization of Psychology in India and its relevance. In V. Kim and J.W. Berry (Eds.), Indigenous psychologies: Research and experience in cultural context (pp. 30-43). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Sinha, D. (1997). Indigenizing psychology. In J. W. Berry, Y. H. Poortinga, and J. Pandey (Eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology, Vol. I – Theory and method, pp. 129-169.Boston: Allyn & Beacon.
Sinha, J. (1936/1958). Indian psychology, volume 1 - Cognition. New Delhi: Motilal Banarasidass.
Sinha, J. (1961). Indian psychology, volume 2 - Emotion and will. New Delhi: Motilal Banarasidass.
Sinha, J. (1968). Indian psychology, volume 3 - Epistemology of perception. New Delhi: Motilal Banarasidass.
Smith, J.A., Harre, R., & Van Langenhove, L. (Ed.). (1995). Rethinking in psychology. London: Thousand Oaks, New Delhi: Sage Publications.
Smith, J.A., Harre, R., & Van Langenhove, L. (Ed.). (1995a). Rethinking methods in psychology. London: Thousand Oaks; New Delhi: Sage Publications.
Sri Aurobindo. (1956). On the Vedas. Pondicherry: Sri Aurobindo International University Centre.
Sri Ramana Maharshi. (1996). The collected works of Ramana Maharshi. Tiruvannamalai: Sri Ramanashramam.
Srivastava, S. P. (2001). Systematic survey of Indian psychology. Bahadurgarh, Haryana: Adhyatma Vijñāna Prakashan.
Taimni, I. K. (1973). Glimpses into the psychology of yoga. Adyar, Madras: The Theosophical Publishing House.
Tart, C. T. (1975a). Transpersonal psychologies. San Francisco: Harper & Row.
Tart, C. T. (1975b). States of consciousness. New York: Dutton.
Tart, C. T. (1969). Altered states of consciousness. New York: Wiley.
Tart, C. T. (1972). States of consciousness and state-specific science. Science, 176, 1203-1210.
Thomas, R.M. (2001). Folk psychologies across cultures. New Delhi: Sage Publications.
Turner, H. M. (1981). Maps of the mind. New York: Macmillan Publishing Company.
Valle, R. S., & von Ekartsberg, R. (1989). Metaphors of consciousness. New York: Plenum Press.
Zimmer, H. (1951). Philosophies of India. (Ed. Campbell). London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Walsh, R. (1980). The consciousness disciplines and the behavioural sciences. American Journal of Psychiatry, 137, 663-673.
Endnotes
1 Italics added for emphasis
2 Hylozoism is the philosophical doctrine holding that all matter has life, which is a property or derivative of matter.
3 Generally, property or characteristic of a thing, but in S??kya philosophy gu?a implies one of the three (sattva, rajas, tamas) “strands” or interacting components of Prak?ti, the primordial materiality of the universe.
4 Late Prof. S.K. Ramachadra Rao was a rare scholar with this kind of unusual combination.
5 Pasyema ?arada?a?am, ?ravye?a ?arad??a?am.
6 Italics added for emphasis
7 Italics author’s
8 In classical Kannada language the phrase asu neegidaru means ‘a person died’ and this phrase is in current usage.
9 Italics added for emphasis
10 Italics added for emphasis
11 Readers may refer to any standard English translation of this Upanishad to get at this.